Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T08:47:23.518Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

TESTING THE FUNDAMENTAL DIFFERENCE HYPOTHESIS

L2 Adult, L2 Child, and L1 Child Comparisons in the Acquisition of Korean Wh-Constructions with Negative Polarity Items

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2009

Hyang Suk Song*
Affiliation:
McGill University
Bonnie D. Schwartz*
Affiliation:
University of Hawai‘i
*
*Address correspondence to: Hyang Suk Song, Department of Linguistics, McGill University, 1085 Dr Penfield Avenue, Montréal, Québec H3A 1A7, Canada; e-mail: hyangsuk.song@mail.mcgill.ca; or Bonnie D. Schwartz, Department of Second Language Studies, University of Hawai‘i, 1890 East-West Rd., Honolulu, HI 96822; e-mail: bds@hawaii.edu.
*Address correspondence to: Hyang Suk Song, Department of Linguistics, McGill University, 1085 Dr Penfield Avenue, Montréal, Québec H3A 1A7, Canada; e-mail: hyangsuk.song@mail.mcgill.ca; or Bonnie D. Schwartz, Department of Second Language Studies, University of Hawai‘i, 1890 East-West Rd., Honolulu, HI 96822; e-mail: bds@hawaii.edu.

Abstract

The fundamental difference hypothesis (FDH; Bley-Vroman, 1989, 1990) contends that the nature of language in natives is fundamentally different from the nature of language in adult nonnatives. This study tests the FDH in two ways: (a) via second language (L2) poverty-of-the-stimulus (POS) problems (e.g., Schwartz & Sprouse, 2000) and (b) via a comparison between adult and child L2 learners, whose first language (L1) is the same, in terms of their developmental route (e.g., Schwartz, 1992, 2003). The phenomena under investigation are Korean wh-constructions with negative polarity items (NPIs). Korean has subject (S)-object (O)-verb (V) as its canonical word order and it is also a wh-in-situ language, but scrambling of the object to presubject position (i.e., movement that results in OSV word order) is generally optional; however, in the context of negative questions with a NPI subject (e.g., amwuto “anyone”), (a) object wh-phrases must scramble on the wh-question reading and (b) the nonscrambled order has a yes/no-question reading. These properties of Korean wh-constructions with NPIs constitute POS problems for nonnatives whose L1 is English (as well as for native Korean-acquiring children). L1-English adult L2 learners (n = 15) and L1-English child L2 learners (n = 10), independently assessed for Korean proficiency, as well as L1-Korean children (n = 23) and L1-Korean adults (n = 15) completed an elicited-production task, an acceptability-judgment task, and an interpretation-verification task. The results show that (a) high-proficiency (adult and child) L2 learners performed like the native adult controls on all three tasks, thereby demonstrating L2 POS effects; and (b) adult and child L2 learners follow the same (inferred) route to convergence, a route differing from—yet subsuming—the L1-child route. Both sets of results lead us to conclude that, contra the FDH, the nature of language is fundamentally similar in natives and (adult or child) nonnatives.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aoun, J., & Li, Y. A. (1993). Syntax of scope. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Beck, S. (1996). Quantified structures as barriers for LF movement. Natural Language Semantics, 4, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, S. (2006). Intervention effects follow focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 14, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, S., & Kim, S.-S. (1997). On wh- and operator scope in Korean. Journal of East Asian Languages, 6, 339384.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1989). What is the logical problem of foreign language learning? In Gass, S. M. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 4168). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1990). The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis, 20, 349.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S. (1997). On the typology of wh-questions. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Cho, S. W. (1981). The acquisition of word order in Korean. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Calgary, Canada.Google Scholar
Choi, Y.-S. (2007). Intervention effect in Korean wh-questions: Indefinite and beyond. Lingua, 117, 20552076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, G. (1994). Case and its acquisition in Korean. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Muysken, P. (1986). The availability of Universal Grammar to adult and child learners: A study of the acquisition of German word order. Second Language Research, 2, 93119.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Muysken, P. (1989). The UG paradox in L2 acquisition. Second Language Research, 5, 129.Google Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., Sprouse, R. A., & Thyre, R. (1999/2000). Quantification at a distance in English-French interlanguage: Domain specificity and second-language acquisition. Language Acquisition, 8, 265320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ha, S. (2008). The existential reading of WH-words and their scope relations. In Adams, N., Cooper, A., Parrill, F., & Wier, T. (Eds.), Proceedings from the 40th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (Vol. 1, pp. 8395). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Hoji, H. (1985). Logical form constraints and configurational structures in Japanese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. (2005). Constraining second language word order optionality: Scrambling in advanced English-German and Japanese-German interlanguage. Second Language Research, 21, 3472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in school children and adults. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 35, 163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iwashita, N. (2006). Syntactic complexity measures and their relations to oral proficiency in Japanese as a foreign language. Language Assessment Quarterly, 3, 151169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1991). Critical period effects on universal properties: The status of subjacency in the acquisition of a second language. Cognition, 39, 215258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanno, K. (1998). The stability of UG principles in second language acquisition: Evidence from Japanese. Linguistics, 36, 11251146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S., O’Grady, W., & Cho, S. (1995). The acquisition of case and word order in Korean: A note on the role of context. Language Research, 31, 687696.Google Scholar
Kim, S.-S. (2002). Intervention effects are focus effects. In David, J. S. (Ed.), Proceedings of Japanese-Korean linguistics (Vol. 8, pp. 615628). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Lakshmanan, U. (1995). Child second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 301329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lalleman, J. (1986). Dutch language proficiency of Turkish children born in the Netherlands. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsden, H. (2003). Inverse scope in L2 Japanese. In Beachly, B., Brown, A., & Conlin, F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 2, pp. 496507). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Miyagawa, S. (2003). A-movement scrambling and options without optionality. In Karimi, S. (Ed.), Word order and scrambling (pp. 177200). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishigauchi, T. (1990). Quantification in the theory of grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortega, L. (2003). Syntactic complexity measures and their relationship to L2 proficiency: A research synthesis of college-level L2 writing. Applied Linguistics, 4, 492518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pesetsky, D. (2000). Phrasal movement and its kin. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (1987). The modular basis of second language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (1990). Un-motivating the motivation for the fundamental difference hypothesis. In Burmeister, H. & Rounds, P. (Eds.), Variability in second language acquisition (pp. 667684). Eugene: University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (1992). Testing between UG-based and problem-solving models of L2A: Developmental sequence data. Language Acquisition, 2, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (2003). Child L2 acquisition: Paving the way. In Beachly, B., Brown, A., & Conlin, F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, pp. 2650). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D. (2004). Why child L2 acquisition? In van Kampen, J. & Baauw, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition 2003 (Vol. 1, pp. 4766). Utrecht, The Netherlands: LOT.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access model. Second Language Research, 12, 4072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (2000). When syntactic theories evolve: Consequences for L2 acquisition research. In Archibald, J. (Ed.), Second language acquisition and linguistic theory (pp. 156186). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sells, P. (2001). Negative polarity licensing and interpretations. In Kuno, S., Lee, I., Whitman, J., Maling, J., Kang, Y., & Kim, Y. (Eds.), Harvard studies in Korean linguistics (Vol. 9, pp. 322). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2001). Telicity in the second language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, H. S. (2007). L2 knowledge of the intervention effect in English-speaking learners of Korean. In Bamman, D., Magnitskaia, T., & Zaller, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development supplement. Retrieved March 17, 2007, from http://www.bu.edu/linguistics/APPLIED/BUCLD/supp31.html.Google Scholar
Song, H. S. (2008). On the L2 acquisition of Korean wh-constructions with negative polarity items: Adult L2, child L2, and child L1 development. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Hawai‘i.Google Scholar
Song, H. S., & Schwartz, B. D. (2008, November). On the acquisition of Korean wh-constructions with negative polarity items. Paper presented at the 18th Japanese/Korean Linguistics Conference (JK18), City University of New York (CUNY).Google Scholar
Tanaka, H. (2003). Remarks on Beck’s effects: Linearity in syntax. Linguistic Inquiry, 34, 314323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomioka, S. (2007). Pragmatics of LF intervention effects: Japanese and Korean wh-interrogatives. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 15701590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2005). Child L2, adult L2, child L1: Differences and similarities—A study on the acquisition of direct object scrambling in Dutch. Utrecht, The Netherlands: LOT.Google Scholar
Whong-Barr, M., & Schwartz, B. D. (2002). Morphological and syntactic transfer in child L2 acquisition of the English dative alternation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 579616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar