Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T08:55:01.566Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Complexity and Nonlinear Dynamics in Psychotherapy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2009

Günter Schiepek
Affiliation:
Institute of Synergetics and Psychotherapy Research, Paracelsus Medical University, Christian Doppler Universitätsklinikum, Ignaz Harrer Str. 79, A-5020 Salzburg, Austria. E-mail: guenter.schiepek@ccsys.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Human life changes with time. It seems therefore obvious that most of the phenomena that psychology and psychotherapy are concerned with are dynamic in nature. For human development processes, human change and learning processes, the dynamics and prognosis of mental disorders, problems manifesting in social systems such as couples, families, teams, or the question of how psychotherapy works, self-organization is ubiquitous. In the context of self-organization, complexity is a quality of changing patterns and patterns of change, produced by nonlinear coupled systems.

Type
Focus: Complexity
Copyright
Copyright © Academia Europaea 2009

Anomalies of the linear input–output model of psychotherapy

A huge amount of empirical outcome studies and reviews (meta-analyses) supports the evidence that psychotherapy works – its efficacy is comparable to other medical treatments. The usual conception is that it works due to the applied methods of treatment. The input makes the output, and the treatment causes the effect. The specific factors producing the effect should exist within the treatment techniques; everything else is used only to fill in the blanks and is therefore non-specific. Against this background, for the purpose of proving the efficiency of psychotherapies, the execution of randomized controlled trials is regarded as the ‘gold standard’. It makes use of experimental designs where treatment systematically varies and confusing variables are to be excluded (e.g. by randomization of patients to treatment modalities). The effect should be attributed to the employed treatment as clearly as possible. To describe this dominating paradigm, WampoldReference Wampold1 used the term ‘medical model’, or – since it also dominates in the field of psychology as well and corresponds to the methodological canon of the general linear model – ‘standard model’.

The model is highly plausible and seems to be confirmed anew with every effective therapy. But it has a blemish – it is falsified. Falsified in a sense that within empirical sciences where truth values of 0 or 1 basically cannot occur, but only more or less confirmed hypotheses, more or less accepted arguments, and more or less empirical ‘corroboration’,Reference Popper2 things sometimes prove ‘falsified’. After 50 years of process-outcome research, the anomalies of the standard model are considerable.

  1. 1. Treatment techniques and technique-specific factors of effectiveness explain only a small portion of the outcome variance. Estimations performed on the basis of meta-analyses lie between 15% and 1% of the explained variance for the therapy outcome.Reference Wampold1, Reference Beutler, Malik, Alimohamed, Harwood, Talebi, Noble and Wong3Reference Lambert and Ogles5 If the intervention, especially if it corresponds to the diagnosis, would determine the outcome, this should not be so, and it also should not be that practically all therapeutic procedures, tested against each other in direct experimental comparisons, lead to very similar effects (the so-called Dodo bird effect).Reference Lambert and Ogles5Reference Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, Benson and Ahn10 This is particularly true when (i) only bona fide therapies were included, i.e. treatment procedures that are well-intentioned and realized by competent therapists as probably effective treatment methods (not only as half-hearted control conditions); and when (ii) the ‘affiliation’, i.e. the identification of practitioners and examiners with their methods and the consequential methodical biases controlled.Reference Luborsky, Diguer, Seligman, Rosenthal, Krause, Johnson, Halperin, Bishop, Berman and Schweizer7 According to corresponding meta-analyses, comparable effects are to be presumed even when specific treatments are suitably used for specific diagnoses – e.g. IPT (Interpersonal Psychotherapy) or CBT (Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy) in case of depression.Reference Wampold1

  2. 2. Early rapid responses. Specific effects can occur even before the specific treatment components were realized and brought into action in a treatment program.Reference Haas, Hill, Lambert and Morrell11Reference Wilson16 For example: cognitive restructuring takes place before the beginning of irrational beliefs treatment in cognitive behavioral therapy, or reduction of compulsions before the flooding begins.

  3. 3. Dismantling studies. The components of treatment programmes can be used in various ways, in different orders and, for the most part, even omitted without essentially impairing the effects (e.g. on cognitive behavioral therapy, see Ref. Reference Jacobson, Dobson, Truax, Addis, Koerner, Gollan, Gortner and Prince17; for reviews see Refs Reference Wampold1, Reference Lambert and Ogles5 andReference Ahn and Wampold18).

  4. 4. Efficacy of non-professionals and paraprofessionals. Laymen, i.e. people with no specific expert knowledge and psychotherapy training, and paraprofessionals, i.e. people active in the field of health care, but with no specific psychotherapy training, are astonishingly successful. In direct comparisons, some meta-analyses credit them with the same efficacy as that of professional psychotherapists.Reference Gunzelmann, Schiepek and Reinecker19, Reference Hattie, Sharpley and Rogers20 Similar conclusions are drawn by the findings that show hardly any, or only very little, connection between training (i.e. extent of training) and treatment results (e.g. Ref. Reference Anderson21), although the results are not uniform.Reference Lambert and Ogles5 While considering whether this should be interpreted as an argument for the importance of natural social competencies or other conditions, it is certain that laymen do not master specific treatment techniques.

  5. 5. Placebo effects. It has often been discussed whether the term placebo – apart from pharmacotherapy, where one can justifiably distinguish between verum and placebo – makes any sense within psychological treatments. If we understand it as a non-specific attentiveness offered to patients in control group conditions to distinguish between treatment and non-specific effects, it shows that bona fide psychotherapies are more effective than placebo conditions (those who equate therapy effects with placebo effect are thus simplifying), but placebo conditions also produce clearly better results than no treatment or waiting-list control groups.Reference Grissom22

  6. 6. Manualized therapies do not show any convincing successes,Reference Wampold1, Reference Beutler, Malik, Alimohamed, Harwood, Talebi, Noble and Wong3 and highly structured therapies are no more effective than those designed by therapists in naturalistic settings.Reference Lipsey and Wilson23, Reference Shadish, Navarro, Matt and Phillips24 The skills described and given in manuals can be learned, but do not necessarily make better therapists.Reference Beutler, Malik, Alimohamed, Harwood, Talebi, Noble and Wong3, Reference Ogles, Anderson and Lunnen25 Neither compliance nor adherence (treatment integrity) correlates continuously high with outcome. On the contrary, sometimes manual integrity seems to undermine the natural social competencies of therapists.Reference Henry, Schacht, Strupp, Butler and Binder26, Reference Henry, Strupp, Butler, Schacht and Binder27 Manuals mainly appear useful to beginners. Therapists obviously also wait for successes to appear, before actually employing the manual. In any case, the execution of manuals must leave space for therapeutic experience, competencies, and justified deviations.

  7. 7. Non-specific factors. Numerous studies have pointed out the importance of variables outside interventions and technical components of psychotherapy, e.g. the therapeutic relationship and working alliance, patient’s expectations and attitudes concerning therapy, the fitting of patient’s subjective disease theories and therapist’s treatment theory, therapist’s authenticity and genuineness, conditions in the social environment of the patient (partnership, family, occupation), setting variables, and so on.Reference Grencavage and Norcross28, Reference Weinberger29

  8. 8. Patient variables seem to have the most important impact on the therapy outcome.Reference Clarkin and Levy30 More than about the diagnosis and problem severity it is about the premorbid adjustment (personality disorders are considered an unfavourable predictor), personal and interpersonal competencies and resources, and particularly about the patient’s intrinsic motivation to change, his self-relatedness, and receptivity to change.

  9. 9. Research findings on self-organization and nonlinear dynamics. Patient and therapist variables unfold in a dynamic and ever-changing context, i.e. interactively, and by this they take effect and produce emergent qualities (Ref. Reference Clarkin and Levy30, p. 215). Orlinsky and Howard’s generic modelReference Orlinsky and Howard31, Reference Orlinsky, Ronnestad and Willutzki32 can be regarded as a synopsis of the variables to create these system effects. Various studies have explicitly examined the nonlinear and non-stationary dynamics of a complex therapy system on an individual level (for psychological measurements see Ref. Reference Haken and Schiepek33; for physiological measurements see Ref. Reference Perlitz, Cotuk, Lambertz, Grebe, Schiepek, Petzold, Schmid-Schönbein and Flatten34), on the level of micromatching between the therapist and patient, or on the level of interpersonal processes at a hospital ward (see the following sections of this contribution). Empirical evidence suggests that therapy works in cascades of transitions between bio-psycho-social patterns at different time scales.

  10. 10. The brain does not function as a serial input–output system. Suitable computer metaphors of brain functioning are obsolete.Reference Mainzer35, Reference Mainzer36 It is much more about a complex, self-organizing system, in which nonlinearities determine the functioning within neurons, and even more so between neurons and neural assemblies (e.g. mixed feedback loops, nonlinear coupled oscillators).Reference Haken and Schiepek33, Reference Freeman37Reference Osipov, Kurths and Zhou40 There are nonlinear resonance and coupling effects which build the basis of healthy (e.g. binding processesReference Singer and Gray41) as well as pathological neural processes (over-synchronizationReference Popovych, Hauptmann and Tass42).

These phenomena and empirical findings can be seen as anomalies of the linear input–output model of psychological treatments. Opposing this ‘standard model’, they support the hypothesis of nonlinearity and self-organization in human change processes. Additionally, they suggest that psychotherapy would not represent an execution of interventions in the sense of environmental events, by which the system is to be forced to a particular reaction, but a concept of psychotherapy as the dynamical realization of conditions for the possibility of human self-organization within a certain professional context.

Nonlinear dynamics in the interaction of patient and therapist

As several research programmes revealed during the last few decades, psychological change processes show all important features of nonlinear systems – such as deterministic chaos, non-stationary phase transitions, and nonlinear coupling between patient and therapist. Physiological synchronization appears to be realized at an interpersonal level (between therapist and patient) as well as between different phenomenological levels of the interpersonal system (speech qualities and psycho-physiological variables). In a study by Villmann et al.Reference Villmann, Liebers, Bergmann, Gumz and Geyer43 heart rate, respiratory frequency, muscular tension, and skin conductivity were measured from both therapist and patient, during 37 therapy sessions. Speech production was analysed by the Mergenthaler Model, focusing on emotional feeling and cognitive referential activity/abstraction.Reference Mergenthaler44 Physiological data were analysed by an artificial neural network approach (growing self-organizing map), which used a kernel smoothing for improved data density estimation. It was possible to generate an entropy model of psycho-physiological variability detecting emotionally unstable phases during the therapy process. The entropy reflecting psycho-physiological and emotional variability was related to the dramatic value of speech analysis according to the cycle model of Mergenthaler.

Empirical evidence also exists for synchronized chaoto-chaotic phase transitions in the brains of therapist and patient during a therapeutic interview, measured by Local Largest Lyapunov Exponents in the EEGs of both interaction partners.Reference Rockstroh, Watzl, Kowalik, Cohen, Sterr, Müller and Elbert45

Taking into account the importance of the therapeutic relationship for the treatment outcome, a study of our own group focused on the interactional process between therapist and patient.Reference Kowalik, Schiepek, Kumpf, Roberts and Elbert46Reference Strunk and Schiepek48 The study applied the method of Sequential Plan Analysis, which is a development of the hierarchical plan analysis proposed by Grawe and Caspar (e.g. Ref. Reference Caspar49). Plans in this sense are verbally or non-verbally communicated intentions of self-presentation in a social situation. The patient’s and therapist’s interactional behaviour was analysed on the basis of video recordings. Two complete therapies (13 and 9 therapy sessions, respectively) were encoded with a sampling rate of 10 seconds (Figure 1). The construction of an inclusive hierarchical plan-analysis leads to an idiographic categorical system for the observation of the client-therapist interaction (Table 1).

Figure 1 Nominal sequences of interactional plans of the therapist (top) and the patient (bottom) during a psychotherapy session. The sampling rate is 10 s. Different plans can be realized simultaneously. The pattern looks like a music score with the plans representing the different instruments of an orchestra. A sonification of the score of plans coded from a 13-session psychotherapy is recorded on a DVD added to the textbook of Haken and SchiepekReference Haken and Schiepek33

Table 1 Second-order plans and categories of self-presentations as identified by the hierarchical plan analysis of a complete 13-session psychotherapy. Encoding of therapist and patient. Plans and categories are used as idiographic observation categories for the Sequential Plan Analysis

The first hints of order in the dynamics came from the distribution of simultaneous (vertical) configurations (on–off patterns) of plans in the scores. This distribution follows a power law (1/f a) demonstrating a distinct order within the data. Following Bak et al.,Reference Bak, Chen and Creutz50 power law-distributions emerge from self-organized criticality of dynamic systems.

Further data analysis was based on the time series of the highest-level categories, the so-called categories of self-presentation (see Table 1). Since in the hierarchical system of the plan analysis, the operators at the lowest observation level were quantified by intensity ratings, the plans and the self-presentation categories at the top level integrating the lower level categories were also quantified (Figure 2).

Figure 2 Time series of the categories of self-representation. From top to bottom (T = therapist; P = patient): TI encourage trust/create a secure atmosphere; TII confrontation/exposing to insecurity; TIII encourage self-responsibility of the patient; TIV activate structuring work; PI search for sympathy/appreciation/good relationship; PII externalization/demonstration of helplessness; PIII problem-oriented work (self-relatedness versus avoidance)

The time series were analysed by methods that are sensitive to the nonlinearity as well as the non-stationarity of the time series.Reference Haken and Schiepek33, Reference Kowalik, Schiepek, Kumpf, Roberts and Elbert46Reference Strunk and Schiepek48 Nonlinearity was proved by surrogate data testsReference Rapp, Albano, Zimmerman and Jiménez-Montaño51 using random surrogates as well as FFT-based phase-randomized surrogates.Reference Strunk52 Whereas fractal dimensionalities of the empirical time series (based on the correlation dimension D2 as well as the mean Pointwise D2 [PD2]Reference Skinner, Molnar and Tomberg53) saturated at finite values (convergence to a fractal dimensionality of about 6), random and FFT-surrogates did not. The methods of PD2Reference Skinner, Molnar and Tomberg53 and of the Local Largest Lyapunov Exponents (algorithm from Ref. Reference Rosenstein, Collins and de Luca54) were used to identify phase-transition-like discontinuities. Following the evolution of PD2 dimensionalities, both therapies realized non-stationarities, and both therapies showed periods of strongly synchronized (with correlations from 0.80 to 1.00) and anti-synchronized PD2-processes (with correlations from −0.80 to −1.00) between patient and therapist. Quite similar and even more pronounced dynamical jumps were to be seen in the development of the Local Largest Lyapunov Exponents (Figure 3), representing changes in the chaoticity of a time signal.Reference Kowalik, Schiepek, Kumpf, Roberts and Elbert46 An important part of the discontinuities of the LLLE were exactly synchronized between patient and therapist. Obviously both persons create a dynamic self-organizing communication system, which can be seen as the trigger system for the individual change processes of the patient.

Figure 3 Synchronized jumps in the dynamics of Local Largest Lyapunov Exponents (black arrows). Grey arrows indicate not clearly synchronized changes. Top: therapist, bottom: patient

These results get support from nonlinear coupling measures between the time series of the interaction partners. Pointwise Transinformation as well as Pointwise Coupling Conditional DivergenceReference Vandenhouten55, Reference Lambertz, Vandenhouten and Langhorst56 were applied to the data, and both indicate changing and time-dependent coupling strengths between the time series of the interaction partners. There is no priority to the therapist’s influence on the patient, which contradicts the classical idea that input from the therapist should determine the client’s output. The other way round is also true and both constitute the circular causality of psychotherapeutic self-organization.

Self-organization in human change processes

A quite different approach to human change processes focused on inpatient treatments at a hospital of psychosomatics. In a study (results in Ref. Reference Haken and Schiepek33) 94 change processes were investigated, realized by 91 inpatients with different diagnoses (depression, anxiety disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, eating disorder, somatoform disorder, and others). Here, the time series data were produced by patients’ self-ratings given a day in the evening. For this purpose a 53-item rating sheet was developed (Therapy Process Questionnaire [TPQ] Reference Haken and Schiepek33) whose factor structure is represented in Table 2. The ratings combined seven-step Likert scales and visual analogue scales, especially for ratings of emotions. TPQ measurements reflect important aspects of the patient’s experience of progress and goal attainment, emotional involvement, self-efficacy, therapeutic relationship, social relations with other inpatients, and the ward atmosphere. The inclusive outcome criterion integrated different measures (questionnaires as well as ratings of the therapist).

Table 2 Factors (principal component analysis) of the Therapy Process Questionnaire (TPQ). Factor analysis was based on 94 therapy processes (mean stay = 66 days, daily ratings). Seven first order factors (right) are related to three second order factors (left). Numbers behind the first-order factors indicate factor loadings on second-order factors (for details see Ref. Reference Haken and Schiepek33)

Results confirmed synergetic conceptualizations of how psychotherapy works and corroborated hypotheses drawn from self-organization theory. Here, therapy is supposed to provide support for the patient’s own self-organization processes, which should be characterized by cascades of order-to-order transitions accompanied by critical instabilities of the process. Pathological and restrictive order should be transformed into more flexible and adaptive patterns of behaviour, and the synchronization of the different aspects of the patient’s experience should undergo some transformations. Exactly this could be observed.

Significant correlations exist between the local maxima of dynamic complexity57 and the outcome of psychotherapy. The local maxima were defined by the difference between the mean dynamic complexity of the whole psychotherapy process and the maximum of the complexity that was observed during the process (data were created by daily applications of the Therapy Process Questionnaire TPQ). Correlations were −0.455 (second order factor I: ‘change involvement’ of the TPQ, p = 0.002), −0.431 (second order factor II: ‘relationship/social climate’, p = 0.003), and −0.572 (second order factor III: ‘emotionality’, p = 0.000) (compare Table 2). Negative correlations result from the fact that increased local maxima of dynamic complexity correspond to more reduced problems, symptoms, and impairment.

In order to answer the question of whether the observed intensities of dynamic complexity reach critical values, intra-item calibration procedures were used in order to define adequate thresholds fitting to the actual dynamics. The time series of dynamic complexity were standardized by z-transformations, providing significance thresholds of 5% or 1%. Applying this threshold method to all items of the TPQ reduces the quantitative complexity signals of each time series to a three-step signal (not significant, complexity exceeds a 5% threshold, complexity exceeds a 1% threshold). A synopsis of these qualitative signals referring to all items of the TPQ gives an impression of the localization of critical fluctuations during the whole process. Dynamic complexities seem to be synchronized over many items and factors of the TPQ, resulting in columns of grey (<5%) or black (<1%) dots. In a large part of the investigated therapies, such column-like structures could be identified. In an item-by-time synopsis they indicate phases of intensified as well as synchronized fluctuations and entropies of quite different aspects of the process. Consequently, these item-by-time synopses are called complexity resonance diagrams (Figure 4).

Figure 4 Complexity resonance diagram of a psychotherapy process. Such diagrams portray the threshold exceeding dynamic complexities of a process encoded by the 53 items of the Therapy Process Questionnaire (TPQ). Gray dots: 5% threshold of significance; black dots: 1% threshold of significance. x-axis: days of hospital stay, y-axis: items of the TPQ arranged by the order of the factors as reported in Table 2. Window width for the calculation of dynamic complexities is 7. Column-like structures indicate phases of critical instabilities during the process

In order to confirm the structures found within the complexity resonance diagrams, surrogate tests were realized based on random as well as on FFT-based surrogates of the time series. The empirical patterns are impressively different from the surrogate-based patterns (all realized comparisons with p = 0.000). Further support for phase-transition like phenomena in the change processes came from Recurrence Plots representing similarities and dissimilarities of dynamic segments of a whole time series.Reference Vandenhouten55, Reference Eckmann, Oliffson Kamphorst and Ruelle58, Reference Webber and Zbilut59 This method is based on the embedding of time series into a phase space constructed by time-delay coordinates, a method that is also crucial in the algorithms for the estimation of dimensional complexity or chaoticity (e.g. Kolmogorov-Sinai-Entropy, Lyapunov Exponents). Neighbours in the time-delay phase space represent similar dynamic segments and are plotted by a dot in the Recurrence Plot. Dissimilarities are represented by empty columns in the Recurrence Plots, which in many cases exactly correspond to the columns of dots in the complexity resonance diagrams (see fig. 6). The overall correlation is −0.45, if small shifts (lags of ±3 measurement points at maximum) will be allowed. This means that periods of critical instability correspond to transient dynamics outside of the quasi-attractors established by the self-organizing system under consideration.

These different ways to identify critical phase transitions are further validated by the Time Frequency Distribution of the time series. The TFD method uses wavelet spectra in order to scan the evolution of the frequency distributions within a signal.Reference Vandenhouten55, Reference Lambertz, Vandenhouten and Langhorst56 It is a dynamic counterpart to the static Fast Fourier Transformation and allows for the identification of pronounced frequency amplitudes or changes in the frequency distributions. In the data set of the referred study, these often appear exactly during the phase transitions, which can be identified by other methods (see the synoptical representations of different time series analysis methods on the DVD in the textbook of Haken & SchiepekReference Haken and Schiepek33).

An overall result of the study is shown in Figure 5. It portrays the evidence that, in order to bring forth change processes within self-organizing systems, at least two conditions should be realized. The first condition is that the degree of the control parameter energizing the system and pushing it away from its actual equilibrium state should exceed a certain intensity level. With respect to psychotherapies, this control parameter could be the patient’s intrinsic motivation to change, including his or her engagement into the therapeutic work. The second condition is the degree of instability the system attains during its change process. This instability during emerging symmetries and symmetry breaking transitions is given by the local maximum of dynamic complexity during the therapy process. The interaction of both conditions results in treatment effectiveness. A third important condition is not represented in Figure 5: it is the experienced stability of the boundary conditions. Boundary conditions can be the outer environment (the context in the ward or therapeutic bond) or the inner environment (intra-systemic conditions such as self-esteem, self-confidence, or activated resources). This context of stability is a prerequisite for a system to undergo critical instabilities.

Figure 5 The effect size (ES) (mean ES of all outcome measures introduced in the study, see text) of inpatient psychotherapy is produced by an interaction between the local maximum of critical fluctuations and the intensity of the control parameter realized during the change process. The local maxima of fluctuations were defined by the difference between the mean dynamic complexity of the whole therapy process and the maximum of the complexity observed during the process. The diagram is based on the mean of the local maxima of all items. The control parameter was defined by the overall mean of the TPQ factor VI: Intensity of therapeutic work/motivation to change

The concept of self-organization promotes new information technologies – the Synergetic Navigation System

Since self-organization and nonlinear dynamics seem to be ubiquitous in human change processes, it should be helpful to go beyond the diagnostics of steady states to an assessment of dynamics. Practitioners should get information on the therapy and its features during the ongoing process in order to use this information for an adequate placement of interventions and a control of the dynamics. ‘Controlling’ self-organization processes in psychotherapies means the generation and co-creation (together with the patient) of adequate boundary conditions, the decision to do or to retain certain interventions, and to support the dynamics that the system is creating by itself. The patient takes an active and cooperative role in this understanding of data-based and co-creative change processes.

Another important reason for the development of real-time assessment comes from the evidence that most of the empirically identified specific and non-specific factors driving therapeutic change processes are connected with specific persons (the concrete therapist who meets a concrete patient) in a concrete context and evolve by its own nonlinear interactions in specific systems. These factors are (i) personal features of the patient, such as his motivation to change, his premorbid adaptation and degree of social functioning, personality integration, ego-strength, or comorbidities; (ii) personal and professional features of the therapist, such as his own personality, social and professional competencies, allegiance to his approach of doing therapy, stress-resistance, and so on; and (iii) factors of the professional and social context (see the so-called Generic Model of psychotherapyReference Lambert and Ogles5, Reference Orlinsky, Ronnestad and Willutzki32). In consequence, evidence-based treatments should be based on the evidence of concrete data mirroring the ongoing change process and on the professional decisions reflecting this insight.

Real-time monitoring actually uses internet-based presentations (including Mini-Laptop or Handy technology) of outcome and process questionnaires. Data are sent to a server, where they are stored and analysed. Professionals and patients can inspect the results whenever they want. Experiences with real-time feedback to therapists (based on an Outcome Questionnaire the patient fills out during the therapy sessions in an ambulatory or outpatient setting) are encouraging. Lambert and co-workers (e.g. Ref. Reference Lambert, Whipple, Vermeersch, Smart, Hawkins, Nielsen and Goates60) were able to identify processes when getting difficult or unsuccessful (‘not on track’ therapies, compared with more promising ‘on track’ therapies), and helped therapists to correct these not-on-track dynamics by specific interventions. By this, threatening drop-outs could be avoided, bad results could be corrected, and on-track processes could be optimized and even shortened.

More sophisticated than the distinction between ‘on-track’ and ‘not-on-track’ courses is the feedback on self-organization features realized by a system based on synergetics.Reference Haken and Schiepek33 The Synergetic Navigation System, using the Therapy Process Questionnaire (see Table 2) for daily ratings, applies methods from nonlinear time series analysis in order to identify important qualities of the change process. This are

  • stability or instability of the dynamics as represented by the subscales (factors) of the TPQ, which is measured by the dynamic complexity;

  • Recurrence Plots indicating transitions or repeating patterns; and

  • intensity of synchronization and time-dependent synchronization patterns between the items and the factors of the TPQ (realized by the cross-correlations of all items of the TPQ, calculated within a running window).

Figure 6 shows a synopsis of these analysis methods applied to an individual therapeutic change process. Preceding the inspection of all analysis results the raw data series of the items and the time courses of the factors (z-transformed values) are available. Additionally, patients can write an electronic diary after filling out the questionnaire. When looking at the time shown on the computer screen, the diary text is shown within a gliding tip-tool running over the time series. By this, corresponding qualitative and quantitative information completes the picture.

Figure 6 Synopsis of a psychotherapy process as monitored by the Synergetic Navigation System. The time course of the inpatient treatment of a patient with eating disorders portrays a clear-cut phase-transition associated with critical instabilities. Top: recurrence plot of the item ‘Today I was successful to do steps towards my personal goals’. Dots represent recurrent segments of the time series, empty spaces represent transitions. Middle: complexity Resonance Diagram of all items of the TPQ. Different from Figure 4, the intensities of the dynamic complexity of each item is transformed into colours. Items are arranged by the order of the first- and second-order factors of TPQ. Bottom: mean of all inter-item correlations irrespective of the sign (absolute values). This is a measure of the overall synchronization of the patient’s experiences as represented by the items of the TPQ. The correlation structure is shown at four measurement points (days) of the psychotherapy process (t = 4, t = 19, t = 33, t = 46). Grey to black cells of the correlation structure matrices represent high correlations, grey to white cells low correlations

Therapist and patient can use this form of feedback as a basis for repeated conversations, in which the course of treatment and patient’s personal development are reflected and discussed in a form of suitable future steps. It is an ‘eye-level’ therapy, where patients can use and develop their self-management resources. Possible functions of such a continuous process summarization and a feedback concerning a ‘tertium datur’, which in this case consists of measurement results presented on the screen, are shown in Figure 7. Thus, the conditions are created for considering psychotherapy as a management of self-organization processes with an equal contribution of the therapist and the patient.

Figure 7 Functions of repeated internet-based self-evaluations (by the use of a therapy process questionnaire or other usual instruments) and of the computer based feedback for patient and therapist. The existing interactions and intra-individual feedback loops are supplemented by an external source of information, which is specialized for measuring and visualizing the characteristics of nonlinear dynamics and therapeutic self-organization

Do order transitions in psychotherapy correspond to patterns of neural activity?

Synergetic research in psychotherapy is reaching the brain. The aim of an actual fMRI-studyReference Schiepek and Schönfelder61, Reference Schiepek, Tominschek, Karch, Lutz, Mulert, Meindl and Pogarell62 was the investigation of phase-transitions of brain activity and related subjective experiences of patients during their psychotherapy process. Repeated fMRI scans were related to the degree of stability or instability of the ongoing dynamics (measured by the dynamic complexity of daily TPQ-ratings) as well as to the therapy outcome. Real-time monitoring by the Synergetic Navigation System allows for the identification of stable or unstable periods and, through this, for a decision on the appropriate moments of fMRI acquisitions. Three or Four scans were realized during each of the psychotherapy processes of 9 patients. The study included patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) of the washing/contamination fear subtype (DSM IV: 300.3), without any medication or comorbid psychiatric or somatic diagnoses. Patients were matched to healthy controls.63

OCD seems to be an appropriate model system for synergetic studies in psychotherapy, since the pathological order parameter is phenomenologically quite evident, the disease has an obvious and quite stable time course, and therapeutic phase transitions – if they do occur – are easy to be observed. OCD-specific functional neuroanatomy is partially known: Friedlander and DesrocherReference Friedlander and Desrocher64 report on an Executive Dysfunction Model corresponding to the cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical feedback-loops involved in perseverations and compulsions, and on a Modulatory Control Model involved in the pathological mechanisms of anxiety and distress provoking obsessions.

The visual stimulation paradigm of the study used symptom provoking, disgust provoking, and neutral pictures. The disgust and the neutral pictures were taken from the International Affective Picture System, whereas the OCD-related pictures were photographed in the home setting of the patients, showing specific and individual symptom provoking stimuli.

For illustrative purposes, we report on the results of a single case. It is a female patient, whose fMRI scans were taken three times during the 59 days of their hospital stay, at days 9, 30, and 57. The healthy control was also scanned three times at identical time intervals to the patient. The second acquisition was done after an intensive period of critical instability of the TPQ-based time series, but just before the flooding was started. (Flooding or response prevention is an essential therapy technique in the behavioural treatment of OCD, where patients are confronted with symptom provoking stimuli but abstain from performing compulsive rituals.) The instability of the patient’s process was the precursor of an important personal decision to divorce from her husband. (It should be noted that the development of her OCD symptoms was in the context of a long-lasting marital conflict.) This decision was the essential phase transition of the therapy.

Indeed, the most pronounced changes in brain activity occurred from the first to the second fMRI scan, whereas BOLD response differences from the second to the third session were only slight. They perhaps represent the neural correlates of an important personal phase transition related to the resolution of a severe personal (marital) conflict. Because these changes occurred before the flooding procedure was started, this can be seen as indicator of an early rapid response in the therapy.Reference Lambert and Ogles5, Reference Hayes, Feldman, Beevers, Laurenceau, Cardaciotto and Lewis-Smith13, Reference Hayes, Laurenceau, Feldman, Strauss and Cardaciotto14 Additionally, marked alternations in brain activity were to be observed before or during symptom reduction took place (measured by the Y-BOCS), not afterwards.

Alternations in brain activity involved widespread areas, e.g. the medial frontal brain regions including anterior cingulate cortex, superior and middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal and precentral gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, superior parietal lobe, cuneus, thalamus and caudate nucleus in both hemispheres, as well as the right fusiform gyrus (see Figure 8 for an OCD to disgust contrast). Thalamic and basal ganglia activation is part of the frontal-caudate-striatum-thalamus circuitry of OCD. In particular the caudate nucleus takes a role within the executive dysfunction model of compulsions, and its activity has been found to be reduced after treatment (e.g. Refs Reference Nakao, Nakagawa, Yoshiura, Nakatani, Nabeyama, Yoshizato, Kudoh, Tada, Yoshioka, Kawamoto, Togao and Kanba65, Reference Schiepek, Tominschek, Karch, Mulert and Pogarell66).

Figure 8 Brain activation patterns of a patient with OCD during psychotherapy. BOLD signals from a 1.5 Tesla fMRT scanner. Top: first scan (ninth day of hospital stay; x = 0, y = –55, z = –2; p(uncor) < 0.001). Middle: second scan (30th day of hospital stay; x = 8, y = –54, z = 5; p(uncor) < 0.001). Bottom: third scan (57th day of hospital stay; x = 0, y = –85, z = 26; p(uncor) < 0.001). Activations during the presentation of OCD-related pictures compared to activations during the presentation of neutral pictures (OCD > disgust)

The function of the anterior cingulate cortex is interesting with regard to synergetics. The cingulate cortex comprises various functions, such as somatosensoric integration, mediation of affective and cognitive processes, control of attention, and processing of painful stimuli. Additionally, it plays an important role as a conflict monitoring system: it is sensitive to ambiguous or conflicting information,Reference van Veen and Carter67, Reference van Veen and Carter68 is involved in decision making,Reference King-Casas, Tomlin, Anen, Camerer, Quartz and Montague69, Reference Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom and Cohen70 and its activation is predictive to treatment outcome in depression.Reference Mayberg, Brannan, Mahurin, Jerabek, Brickman, Tekell, Silva, McGinnis, Glass, Martin and Fox71 This is true especially for the dorsal (cognitive) structures of the ACC. By this, it could be an indicator of symmetry states of brain functioning characterized by two or more dynamic patterns or attractors in competition. In the present case, the ACC activation at the beginning of the therapy could be either part of the pathology or could be indicative for the critical instability of the cognitive-affective system of the patient, preparing her important decision. The second fMRI measure was conducted during a local minimum of critical fluctuations. If the impressive change in cingulate activation could be attributed to a changed critical symmetry state of the neural self-organization before versus after the phase-transition or to changes in symptom severity cannot be decided within a single case study, but seems to be an interesting question to the present data analysis of the whole sample (nine patients)Reference Karch, Schiepek, Aigner, Tominschek, Dold, Lutz, Mulert, Meindl, Pogarell and Zaudig72 and to further research. Perhaps the fact that during the second fMRI measure the Y-BOCS score was nearly on the same level as during the first measure – only 14% reduction of symptom severity, compared with 50% reduction in dynamic complexity – could be an argument in favour of the instability hypothesis.

The paradigm of self-organization is a very promising approach to psychotherapy as well as other fields of psychology and medicine. Its interdisciplinarity is due to the fact that the laws and principles of self-organization are true for neural, mental, and behavioural processes (and the corresponding data qualities). Interdisciplinary cooperation is underpinned by the unifying terminology as well as by the unifying formalism and modelling tools of synergetics. This opens new perspectives for basic and applied research, but also for the treatment of mental disorders.

Günter Schiepek, Univ.-Prof. Dr. phil. Dr. phil. habil. Director of the Institute Synergetics and Psychotherapy Research at the Paracelsus Medical University Salzburg (Austria) and of the Center for Complex Systems (Stuttgart/Germany). Since 2008 professor at the Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich and at the Paracelsus Medical University Salzburg. Visiting-Professor at the Donau-University Krems and at the Alpen-Adria University Klagenfurt (Austria). Member of the European Academy of Sciences and Arts (Academia Scientiarium et Artium Europaea).

Main topics: Synergetics and dynamics of nonlinear systems in psychology, management, and the neurosciences. Process-outcome-research in psychotherapy. Neuroscience of psychotherapy. Internet-based Real-Time Monitioring of change processes in different fields. Social psychology. Management. Research in the measurement and development of human competencies.

Member of the scientific board of different institutions, communities, and journals. Published 20 books and about 150 papers in international and german scientific journals and readers.

References

References and Notes

1.Wampold, B. E. (2001) The Great Psychotherapy Debate. Models, Methods, and Findings (Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).Google Scholar
2.Popper, K. (1987) Logik der Forschung (Tübingen: JCB Mohr) (7. Aufl.).Google Scholar
3.Beutler, L. E., Malik, M., Alimohamed, S., Harwood, T. M., Talebi, H., Noble, F. and Wong, E. (2004) Therapist variables. In: M. J. Lambert (ed.) Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (New York: Wiley), pp. 227306.Google Scholar
4.Shapiro, D. A., Harper, H., Startup, M., Reynolds, S., Bird, D. and Suokas, A. (1994) The high-water mark of the drug metaphor. A meta-analytic critique of process-outcome research. In: R. L. Russell (ed.) Reassessing Psychotherapy Research (New York: Guilford Press), pp. 135.Google Scholar
5.Lambert, M. J. and Ogles, B. M. (2004) The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy. In: M. J. Lambert (ed.) Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (New York: Wiley), pp. 139193.Google Scholar
6.Gaffan, E. A., Tsaousis, I. and Kemp-Wheeler, S. M. (1995) Researcher allegiance and meta-analysis: the case of cognitive therapy for depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 63, 966980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Luborsky, L., Diguer, L., Seligman, D. A., Rosenthal, R., Krause, E. D., Johnson, S., Halperin, G., Bishop, M., Berman, J. S. and Schweizer, E. (1999) The researcher’s own allegiances: A ‘wild card’ in the comparisons of treatment efficacy. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Practice, 6, 95106.Google Scholar
8.Shoham, V. and Rohrbaugh, M. J. (1999) Beyond allegiance to comparative outcome studies. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract., 6, 120123.Google Scholar
9.Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V. and Miller, T. I. (1980) The Benefits of Psychotherapy (Baltimore MD: John Hopkins University Press).Google Scholar
10.Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., Stich, F., Benson, K. and Ahn, H. (1997) A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: empirically, ‘All must have prizes’. Psychol. Bull., 122, 203215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11.Haas, E., Hill, R., Lambert, M. J. and Morrell, B. (2002) Do early responders to psychotherapy maintain treatment gains? J. Clin. Psychol., 58, 11571172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Hayes, A. M. and Strauss, J. L. (1998) Dynamic systems theory as a paradigm for the study of change in psychotherapy: an application to cognitive therapy for depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 66, 939947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13.Hayes, A. M., Feldman, G. C., Beevers, C. G., Laurenceau, J. P., Cardaciotto, L. A. and Lewis-Smith, J. (2007) Discontinuities and cognitive changes in an exposure-based cognitive therapy for depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychology, 75, 409421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14.Hayes, A. M., Laurenceau, J. P., Feldman, G. C., Strauss, J. L. and Cardaciotto, L. A. (2007) Change is not always linear: The study of nonlinear and discontinuous patterns of change in psychotherapy. Clin. Psychol. Rev., 27, 715723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Ilardi, S. S. and Craighead, W. E. (1994) The role of non-specific factors in cognitive-behavior therapy for depression. Clin. Psychol. Res. Pract., 1, 138156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.Wilson, G. T. (1998) Manual-based treatment and clinical practice. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract., 5, 363375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
17.Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A., Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., Gortner, E. and Prince, S. E. (1996) A component analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatment for depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 64, 295304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Ahn, H. and Wampold, B. E. (2001) Where oh where are the specific ingredients? A meta-analysis of component studies in counselling and psychotherapy. J. Counsel. Psychol., 48, 251257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19.Gunzelmann, T., Schiepek, G. and Reinecker, H. (1987) Laienhelfer in der psychosozialen Versorgung: Meta-Analysen zur differentiellen Effektivität von Laien und professionellen Helfern. Gruppendynamik, 18, 361384.Google Scholar
20.Hattie, J. A., Sharpley, C. F. and Rogers, H. F. (1984) Comparative effectiveness of professional and paraprofessional helpers. Psychol. Bull., 95, 534541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Anderson, T. (1999) Specifying non-‘specifics’ in therapists: the effect of facilitative interpersonal skills in outcome and alliance formation. Paper presented at the 30th annual meeting of the International Society of Psychotherapy Research, Braga, Portugal.Google Scholar
22.Grissom, R. J. (1996) The magical number .7 +−.2: Meta-meta-analysis of the probability of superior outcome in comparisons involving therapy, placebo, and control. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 64, 973982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Lipsey, M. W. and Wilson, D. B. (1993) The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioural treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis. Am. Psychologist, 48, 11811209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Shadish, W. R., Navarro, A. M., Matt, G. E. and Phillips, G. (2000) The effects of psychological therapies under clinically representative conditions: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull., 126, 512529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Ogles, B. M., Anderson, T. and Lunnen, K. M. (1999) The contribution of models and techniques to therapeutic efficacy: contradictions between professional trends and clinical research. In: M. A. Hubble, B. L. Duncan and S. E. Miller (eds), The Heart and Soul of Change: What Works in Therapy (Washington DC: APA), pp. 201225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Henry, W. P., Schacht, T. E., Strupp, H. H., Butler, S. F. and Binder, J. (1993) Effects of training in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy: mediators of therapists’ responses to training. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 61, 441447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Henry, W. P., Strupp, H. H., Butler, S. F., Schacht, T. E. and Binder, J. (1993) Effects of training in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy: changes in therapist’s behavior. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 61, 434440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Grencavage, L. M. and Norcross, J. C. (1990) Where are the commonalities among the therapeutic common factors? Professional Psychol. Res. Practice, 21, 372378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29.Weinberger, J. (1995) Common factors aren’t so common: the common factors dilemma. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract., 2, 4569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30.Clarkin, J. F. and Levy, K. N. (2004) The influence of client variables on psychotherapy. In: M. J. Lambert (ed.) Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (New York: Wiley), pp. 194226.Google Scholar
31.Orlinsky, D. E. and Howard, K. J. (1986) Process and outcome in psychotherapy. In: S. L. Garfield and A. E. Bergin (eds) Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, 3rd edn (New York: Wiley), pp. 311381.Google Scholar
32.Orlinsky, D. E., Ronnestad, M. H. and Willutzki, U. (2004) Fifty years of psychotherapy process-outcome research: Continuity and change. In: M. J. Lambert (ed.) Bergin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (New York: Wiley), pp. 307389.Google Scholar
33.Haken, H. and Schiepek, G. (2006) Synergetik in der Psychologie. Selbstorganisation verstehen und gestalten (Göttingen: Hogrefe).Google Scholar
34.Perlitz, V., Cotuk, B., Lambertz, M., Grebe, R., Schiepek, G., Petzold, E. R., Schmid-Schönbein, H. and Flatten, G. (2004) Coordination dynamics of circulatory and respiratory rhythms during psychomotor relaxation. Autonomic. Neurosci., 115, 8293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35.Mainzer, K. (ed.) (1999) Komplexe Systeme in Natur und Gesellschaft. Komplexitätsforschung in Deutschland auf dem Weg ins nächste Jahrhundert (Berlin: Springer).Google Scholar
36.Mainzer, K. (2007) Thinking in Complexity. The Complex Dynamics of Matter, Mind, and Mankind, 4th edn (Berlin: Springer).Google Scholar
37.Freeman, W. J. (1999) How Brains Make Up Their Minds (London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson).Google Scholar
38.Haken, H. (2002) Brain Dynamics (Berlin: Springer).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39.Kelso, J. A. S. (1995) Dynamic Patterns. The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
40.Osipov, G. V., Kurths, J. and Zhou, C. (2007) Synchronization in Oscillatory Networks (Berlin: Springer).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41.Singer, W. and Gray, C. M. (1995) Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Ann. Rev. Neurosci., 18, 555586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Popovych, O. V., Hauptmann, C. and Tass, P. A. (2006) Control of neural synchrony by nonlinear delayed feedback. Biol. Cybern., 95, 6985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43.Villmann, T., Liebers, C., Bergmann, B., Gumz, A. and Geyer, M. (in press) Investigation of psycho-physiological interactions between patient and therapist during a psychodynamic therapy and their relation to speech in terms of entropy analysis using a neural network approach. New Ideas in Psychology.Google Scholar
44.Mergenthaler, E. (1998) Cycles of emotion-abstraction patterns: A way of practice oriented process research? The British Psychological Society – Psychotherapy Section Newsletter, 24, 1629.Google Scholar
45.Rockstroh, B., Watzl, H., Kowalik, Z. J., Cohen, R., Sterr, A., Müller, M. and Elbert, T. (1997) Dynamical aspects of the EEG in different psychopathological states in an interview situation. A pilot study. Schizophrenia Res., 28, 7785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
46.Kowalik, Z. J., Schiepek, G., Kumpf, K., Roberts, L. E. and Elbert, T. (1997) Psychotherapy as a chaotic process II: The application of nonlinear analysis methods on quasi time series of the client-therapist-interaction: A nonstationary approach. Psychother. Res., 7, 197218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47.Schiepek, G., Kowalik, Z. J., Schütz, A., Köhler, M., Richter, K., Strunk, G., Mühlnickel, W. and Elbert, T. (1997) Psychotherapy as a chaotic process I. Coding the client-therapist-interaction by means of sequential plan analysis and the search for chaos: A stationary approach. Psychother. Res., 7, 173194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
48.Strunk, G. and Schiepek, G. (2006) Systemische Psychologie. Einführung in die komplexen Grundlagen menschlichen Verhaltens (Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag).Google Scholar
49.Caspar, F. (1996) Beziehungen und Probleme verstehen. Eine Einführung in die psychotherapeutische Plananalyse (Bern: Huber).Google Scholar
50.Bak, P., Chen, K. and Creutz, M. (1989) Self-organized criticality and the ‘Game of Life’. Nature, 342, 780782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
51.Rapp, P. E., Albano, M. E., Zimmerman, I. D. and Jiménez-Montaño, M. A. (1994) Phase-randomized surrogates can produce spurious identifications of non-random structure. Physical Letters A, 192, 2733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
52.Strunk, G. (2004) Organisierte Komplexität. Mikroprozess-Analysen der Interaktionsdynamik zweier Psychotherapien mit den Methoden der nichtlinearen Zeitreihenanalyse. Dissertation, Universität Bamberg.Google Scholar
53.Skinner, J. E., Molnar, M. and Tomberg, C. (1994) The point correlation dimension: Performance with nonstationary surrogate data and noise. Int. Physiol. Behav. Sci., 29, 217234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Rosenstein, M. T., Collins, J. J. and de Luca, C. J. (1993) A practical method for calculating Largest Lyapunov Exponents from small data sets. Physica D, 65, 117134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
55.Vandenhouten, R. (1998) Analyse instationärer Zeitreihen komplexer Systeme und Anwendungen in der Physiologie (Achen: Shaker Verlag).Google Scholar
56.Lambertz, M., Vandenhouten, R. and Langhorst, P. (2003) Transiente Kopplungen von Hirnstammneuronen mit Atmung, Herzkreislaufsystem und EEG: Ihre Bedeutung für Ordnungsübergänge in der Psychotherapie. In: G. Schiepek (ed.) Neurobiologie der Psychotherapie (Stuttgart: Schattauer), pp. 302324.Google Scholar
57.The measure of dynamic complexity combines a fluctuation index with a distribution index. The fluctuation index measures the frequency and amplitude of the change rates of a time series between the reversals of the development within a scanning window gliding over the whole time series. For analysis purposes a window width of 7 measurement points (=days) was introduced. The distribution index measures the scattering of realized values within a given scanning window. The more scores are restricted to only narrow intervals of the available scale range, the smaller the distribution index becomes. The score of this index increases as the interval filled by the realized values grows. The algorithm solves the problem of value distribution independently of the scale resolution, the width of the scanning window, and of any combination of these parameters.Google Scholar
58.Eckmann, J. P., Oliffson Kamphorst, S. and Ruelle, D. (1987) Recurrence plots of dynamical systems. Europhysics Letters, 4, 973977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
59.Webber, C. L. and Zbilut, J. P. (1994) Dynamical assessment of physiological systems and states using recurrence plot strategies. J. Appl. Physiol., 76, 965973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60.Lambert, M. J., Whipple, J. L., Vermeersch, D. A., Smart, D. W., Hawkins, E. J., Nielsen, S. L. and Goates, M. (2002) Enhancing psychotherapy outcomes via providing feedback on client progress: a replication. Clin. Psychol. Psychother., 9, 91103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
61.Schiepek, G. and Schönfelder, V. (2007) Musterhafter Wandel. Gehirn & Geist, 10/2007, 5258.Google Scholar
62.Schiepek, G., Tominschek, I., Karch, S., Lutz, J., Mulert, C., Meindl, T. and Pogarell, O. (2008) A controlled single case study with repeated fMRI measures during the treatment of a patient with obsessive-compulsive disorder: testing the nonlinear dynamics approach to psychotherapy. World J. Biol. Psychiatry, e-published 2008, journal in print. DOI:10.1080/15622970802311829Google Scholar
63. Multi-centre study of the Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Clinic of Psychiatry (Dr Susanne Karch, PD Dr Oliver Pogarell, Dr Christoph Mulert), Hospital of Psychosomatic Medicine Windach/Ammersee and Day Treatment Centre Munich/Westend (Dr Igor Tominschek, Dipl. Psych. Stephan Heinzel, Professor Dr Michael Zaudig), University Hospital Vienna/Astria, Clinic of Psychiatry (Professor Dr Martin Aigner, Professor Dr Gerhard Lenz, Dr Markus Dold, Dr Annemarie Unger), MR Centre of Excellence, Medical University Vienna/Austria (Professor Dr Ewald Moser, Dr Christian Windischberger, Dr Florian Gerstl), and the Institute of Synergetics, and Psychotherapy Research, Paracelsus Medical University Salzburg/Austria (Professor Dr Günter Schiepek, head of the project).Google Scholar
64.Friedlander, L. and Desrocher, M. (2006) Neuroimaging studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults and children. Clin. Psychol. Rev., 26, 3249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65.Nakao, T., Nakagawa, A., Yoshiura, T., Nakatani, E., Nabeyama, M., Yoshizato, C., Kudoh, A., Tada, K., Yoshioka, K., Kawamoto, M., Togao, O. and Kanba, S. (2005) Brain activation of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder during neuropsychological and symptom provocation tasks before and after symptom improvement: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry, 57, 901910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66.Schiepek, G., Tominschek, I., Karch, S., Mulert, C. and Pogarell, O. (2007) Neurobiologische Korrelate der Zwangsstörungen – Aktuelle Befunde zur funktionellen Bildgebung. Psychother. Psych. Med., 57, 379394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
67.van Veen, V. and Carter, C. C. (2002) The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiol. Behav., 77, 477482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
68.van Veen, V. and Carter, C. C. (2002) The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. J. Cog. Neurosci., 14, 593602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69.King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R. and Montague, P. R. (2005) Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science, 308, 7883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
70.Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E. and Cohen, J. D. (2003) The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science, 300, 17551758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71.Mayberg, H. S., Brannan, S. K., Mahurin, R. K., Jerabek, P. A., Brickman, J. S., Tekell, J. L., Silva, J. A., McGinnis, S., Glass, T. G., Martin, C. C. and Fox, P. T. (1997) Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport, 8, 10571061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
72.Karch, S., Schiepek, G., Aigner, M., Tominschek, I., Dold, M., Lutz, J., Mulert, C., Meindl, T., Pogarell, O. and Zaudig, M. (in preparation) Synergetics of neural and mental change processes: repeated fMRI measures during the psychotherapy of obsessive-compulsive disorder.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Nominal sequences of interactional plans of the therapist (top) and the patient (bottom) during a psychotherapy session. The sampling rate is 10 s. Different plans can be realized simultaneously. The pattern looks like a music score with the plans representing the different instruments of an orchestra. A sonification of the score of plans coded from a 13-session psychotherapy is recorded on a DVD added to the textbook of Haken and Schiepek33

Figure 1

Table 1 Second-order plans and categories of self-presentations as identified by the hierarchical plan analysis of a complete 13-session psychotherapy. Encoding of therapist and patient. Plans and categories are used as idiographic observation categories for the Sequential Plan Analysis

Figure 2

Figure 2Figure 2 Time series of the categories of self-representation. From top to bottom (T = therapist; P = patient): TI encourage trust/create a secure atmosphere; TII confrontation/exposing to insecurity; TIII encourage self-responsibility of the patient; TIV activate structuring work; PI search for sympathy/appreciation/good relationship; PII externalization/demonstration of helplessness; PIII problem-oriented work (self-relatedness versus avoidance)

Figure 3

Figure 2

Figure 4

Figure 3 Synchronized jumps in the dynamics of Local Largest Lyapunov Exponents (black arrows). Grey arrows indicate not clearly synchronized changes. Top: therapist, bottom: patient

Figure 5

Table 2 Factors (principal component analysis) of the Therapy Process Questionnaire (TPQ). Factor analysis was based on 94 therapy processes (mean stay = 66 days, daily ratings). Seven first order factors (right) are related to three second order factors (left). Numbers behind the first-order factors indicate factor loadings on second-order factors (for details see Ref. 33)

Figure 6

Figure 4 Complexity resonance diagram of a psychotherapy process. Such diagrams portray the threshold exceeding dynamic complexities of a process encoded by the 53 items of the Therapy Process Questionnaire (TPQ). Gray dots: 5% threshold of significance; black dots: 1% threshold of significance. x-axis: days of hospital stay, y-axis: items of the TPQ arranged by the order of the factors as reported in Table 2. Window width for the calculation of dynamic complexities is 7. Column-like structures indicate phases of critical instabilities during the process

Figure 7

Figure 5 The effect size (ES) (mean ES of all outcome measures introduced in the study, see text) of inpatient psychotherapy is produced by an interaction between the local maximum of critical fluctuations and the intensity of the control parameter realized during the change process. The local maxima of fluctuations were defined by the difference between the mean dynamic complexity of the whole therapy process and the maximum of the complexity observed during the process. The diagram is based on the mean of the local maxima of all items. The control parameter was defined by the overall mean of the TPQ factor VI: Intensity of therapeutic work/motivation to change

Figure 8

Figure 6 Synopsis of a psychotherapy process as monitored by the Synergetic Navigation System. The time course of the inpatient treatment of a patient with eating disorders portrays a clear-cut phase-transition associated with critical instabilities. Top: recurrence plot of the item ‘Today I was successful to do steps towards my personal goals’. Dots represent recurrent segments of the time series, empty spaces represent transitions. Middle: complexity Resonance Diagram of all items of the TPQ. Different from Figure 4, the intensities of the dynamic complexity of each item is transformed into colours. Items are arranged by the order of the first- and second-order factors of TPQ. Bottom: mean of all inter-item correlations irrespective of the sign (absolute values). This is a measure of the overall synchronization of the patient’s experiences as represented by the items of the TPQ. The correlation structure is shown at four measurement points (days) of the psychotherapy process (t = 4, t = 19, t = 33, t = 46). Grey to black cells of the correlation structure matrices represent high correlations, grey to white cells low correlations

Figure 9

Figure 7 Functions of repeated internet-based self-evaluations (by the use of a therapy process questionnaire or other usual instruments) and of the computer based feedback for patient and therapist. The existing interactions and intra-individual feedback loops are supplemented by an external source of information, which is specialized for measuring and visualizing the characteristics of nonlinear dynamics and therapeutic self-organization

Figure 10

Figure 8 Brain activation patterns of a patient with OCD during psychotherapy. BOLD signals from a 1.5 Tesla fMRT scanner. Top: first scan (ninth day of hospital stay; x = 0, y = –55, z = –2; p(uncor) < 0.001). Middle: second scan (30th day of hospital stay; x = 8, y = –54, z = 5; p(uncor) < 0.001). Bottom: third scan (57th day of hospital stay; x = 0, y = –85, z = 26; p(uncor) < 0.001). Activations during the presentation of OCD-related pictures compared to activations during the presentation of neutral pictures (OCD > disgust)