Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T17:51:31.732Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Session 5: Nutrition communication The challenge of effective food risk communication

Symposium on ‘The challenge of translating nutrition research into public health nutrition’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

Aileen McGloin*
Affiliation:
National Nutrition Surveillance Centre, UCD School of Public Health and Population Science, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Republic of Ireland
Liam Delaney
Affiliation:
UCD Geary Institute, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Republic of Ireland
Eibhlin Hudson
Affiliation:
UCD Geary Institute, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Republic of Ireland
Pat Wall
Affiliation:
UCD School of Public Health and Population Science, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Republic of Ireland
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Aileen McGloin, fax +353 1 716 3421, email aileen.mcgloin@ucd.ie
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A chronology of food scares combined with a rapid, unchecked, rise in lifestyle-related diseases such as obesity highlights the need for a focus on effective food risk communication. However, food risk communication is highly complex. Many factors will affect its success, including the demeanour and conduct of the source, its transparency, interaction with the public, acknowledgement of risks and timely disclosure. How the message is developed is also important in terms of language, style and pretesting with target audiences, as is the choice of appropriate channels for reaching target audiences. Finally, there are many personal factors that may affect risk perception such as previous experience, knowledge, attitudes and beliefs, personality, psychological factors and socio-demographic factors, many of which remain unexplored. While there is evidence that campaigns that communicate health risk have been associated with behaviour change in relation to major public health and safety issues in the past, it is unknown at this stage whether targeting risk information based on risk-perception segmentation can increase the effectiveness of the messages.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2009

Scientists and regulators have long recognised the need to communicate risk to the public. However, much of the early research in this area was focused on issues such as war, nuclear power, road safety, water safety, chemicals and medicines. Only relatively recently have researchers begun to explore the communication of food risks or have European and national agencies been charged with responsibility for food risk communication. This change in approach followed a plethora of food scares, which began in the late 1980s with the well-publicised occurrence of food poisoning from Salmonella in eggs in the UK and has continued up to the present.

Originally, it was believed that communicating risk would allow individuals to process risk more accurately and thus behave more optimally in relation to their health(Reference Romer and Jamieson1Reference Weinstein, Slovic and Gibson5). It was assumed that education was the correct solution to allow the public to interpret risk more ‘rationally’. However, psychological and sociological research has since shown that lay individuals may process risk quite differently from food experts(Reference Fischhoff, Slovic and Lichtenstein6). While the food experts use technical quantitative methods of risk measurement to assess risk, consumers use a broader approach for risk assessments. In a recent qualitative study conducted in four European countries it was found that formal or ‘scientific’ sources of evidence are rarely mentioned in relation to decisions about food risks(Reference Green, Draper and Dowler7). Instead, participants rely on more practical approaches, including perception of naturalness, taste, smell and appearance.

Taking into account what has been learned from risk-perception research, there has been a change in the approach taken to risk communication. In the past, the public was perceived as a passive receiver of risk information and considered to often misunderstand or misinterpret risk messages. In contrast, it is now recognised that risk communication should involve a process of exchange of information among all those concerned with the risk at hand.

While risk communication has advanced greatly in recent years, its complexity means that there is no single set of recommendations to suit all situations. Many publications and reports have produced guides for best practice in risk communication(Reference Covello, Peters and Wojtecki8Reference Lundgren14), which provide useful reading. However, efforts in food risk communication will vary in their purpose, timing (crisis and non-crisis) and the attributes of the hazard involved, all of which will impact greatly on the communications strategy and its potential for success. The effectiveness is further influenced by the source of the information, the message, the mode of delivery and the characteristics of the receiver (see Fig. 1). The present paper will attempt to review and summarise some of these influencing factors.

Fig. 1. Factors affecting risk communication.

The changing food environment and the development of food risk communication

The roots of risk communication lie in risk-perception research. The most widely accepted model of risk perception is the psychometric model. It was developed in the late 1970s and uses nine explanatory scales including voluntariness, immediacy, uncertainty, dread, controllability, catastrophic potential, severity of consequence, known to science and novelty(Reference Fischhoff, Slovic and Lichtenstein6). The strongest predictors of risk have included ‘dread’, i.e. whether individuals can tolerate living with a risk and think about it calmly, and ‘novelty’, i.e. how precisely the risks were known(Reference Fischhoff, Slovic and Lichtenstein6, Reference Boholm15). More recently, ‘naturalness’ has also been found to explain a substantial amount of variation in risk perception(Reference Sjoberg16).

These factors help to explain why individuals react, or fail to react, to different types of food risks. Recent scares include the discovery of BSE in beef in 1996, dioxins in animal feed in Belgium in 1999, concerns around acrylamide in 2002, Enterobactum sakasaki in baby formula in France in 2004, polychlorinated biphenyls in Scottish salmon in 2004, Sudan Red dye in 2005 and bird ‘flu in 2006. These risks elicited responses ranging from media frenzy to the collapse of production of whole food chains, restriction of trade, limitation of food technology development and even government collapse in the case of dioxins in animal feed in Belgium. However, given the characteristics of these risks and what is known from the psychometric model the public response was somewhat predictable.

During the period corresponding to the recent food scares the rates of diet-related diseases such as obesity and diabetes have increased dramatically, tripling in Europe over the past two decades(17), with major implications for human health. While scientists and public health nutritionists describe this situation as a pandemic, studies have shown that, for example, neither a high fat intake nor a high energy intake(Reference Siegrist, Kellerb and Kiers18) score highly in risk perception and the rise in obesity continues unchecked. Whereas technological and food safety issues appear to elicit a dramatic response, healthy eating advice aimed at improving chronic health does not, because the consequences are not immediately apparent.

The many purposes of risk communication

Given the variety of risks to be addressed, the purpose of food risk communications can vary greatly and includes building trust and consensus, creating awareness, educating, influencing perceptions, attitudes and beliefs, promoting action and changing behaviour. In turn, a variety of strategies will be required to achieve each goal. Pre-crisis communications normally involve proactive strategies to call attention to potential and existing risk issues and provide a platform for discussion and information sharing. They may also aim to illicit behaviour change in relation to a well-known food risk or one with long-term consequences(Reference Scherer19). Communicating during a crisis presents a particularly difficult challenge for risk communicators in maintaining public confidence. Strong emotions, such as fear, anxiety, distrust, anger, outrage, helplessness and frustration(Reference Covello, Peters and Wojtecki8, Reference Sandman, Covello, McCallum and Pavlova20) come to the fore and present serious barriers to effective communication(Reference Covello, Matthes, Bernhardt and Repacholi21, Reference Fischhoff22). Convincing the recipient to accept some level of risk is no easy task. Useful guidelines have recently been issued by the WHO in the Sixth Futures Forum on Crisis Communication, which deal with preparedness, infrastructure, timing, availability, transparency, honesty and media relations(10).

Trust, transparency and uncertainty in risk communication

Increasingly, the risk communications strategies of health and regulatory authorities, often the source of risk communications efforts, have come under intense scrutiny, particular during crises. Disasters such as the BSE crisis in the UK and the dioxin scandal in Belgium have focused attention on the causes of public distrust in these institutions and their risk-management practices(Reference Frewer23). Mistrust in a communicator is a major barrier to effective risk communication(Reference Siegrist and Cvetkovich24) and may render the source less credible than other sources such as the mass media(Reference Verbeke, Viane and Guiot25). Only when trust has been established can other goals, such as raising awareness and behaviour change, be achieved.

Four important determining factors have been observed in establishing trust and they include: caring and empathy; dedication and commitment; competence and expertise; honesty and openness(Reference Slovic26). On the other hand, trust is decreased by perceived disagreement among experts, lack of coordination among risk-management organisations, failure to incorporate public participation, an unwillingness to acknowledge risks, delay in disclosure of information and irresponsibility or negligence in fulfilling risk-management responsibilities(Reference Covello, McCallum and Pavlova12, Reference Chess, Salomone and Hance27). For these reasons, measures such as increased transparency in risk-assessment and -management processes, widespread consultation and stakeholder engagement are now viewed as important aspects of risk communications practice.

The demand for transparency and the focus on the needs of the recipient have had an impact on how messages about food risk are developed and on what is communicated. The need to communicate uncertainty is now an important consideration. In the past, scientific experts have worried that communicating uncertainty would result in public distrust. In fact, the opposite was found to be true; failure to communicate about uncertainty increases public distrust in risk-management strategies(Reference Frewer, Miles and Brennan28), while acknowledging uncertainty increases public confidence(Reference Frewer, Howard and Hedderley29). Authorities that are not completely transparent or that over-simplify the risk messages could also be accused of lying(Reference Bridgeman and Ferguson-Smith30Reference Wynne, Lash, Szerszynski and Wynne33).

Effective translation of scientific messages

Equally, the message itself must be clear, easily understood and take into account the concerns of the public. The numerical expressions and small probabilities used by risk assessors can be difficult for non-scientists to understand. However, translating these terms into every-day language is problematic. A meta-analysis has shown that words such as ‘rare’, ‘unlikely’, ‘frequent’ and ‘probable’ mean different things to different individuals(Reference Theil34). The effectiveness of risk comparisons have also been explored in relation to specific risk; for example, what are the risks now compared with 10 years ago or what are the risks compared with a better-known risk(Reference Bier35). However, these comparisons do not take into account the complexity of decision making for an individual.

Research on the use of appropriate language and style of the communication is limited in relation to food risks. The effectiveness of using verbal expressions compared with numerical expressions has been examined in relation to Rn gas(Reference Johnson, Fisher and Smith36). Also compared was understanding of messages that were conveyed using a ‘command’ or directive approach v. a ‘cajole’ or persuasive approach. It was found that the command approach using verbal expressions increases learning and the numerical expressions result in greater consistency between perceived and objective risk, while the ‘cajole’ verbal version increases the probability of making an appropriate recommendation to a neighbour. Thus, no method was found to be best and the communications format will depend on the aim of the risk communication. It is unknown whether this result is specific to communication about Rn gas or whether it is transferable to food risks. Again, this uncertainty highlights the need for pre-testing messages with key audiences.

Communicating both benefits and risks

The scenario in which a certain behaviour presents both risks and benefits presents an interesting case study. Such a situation has recently been reviewed in relation to oily fish, which provide the benefit of n-3 fatty acids, but may also be contaminated with heavy metals(Reference Verbeke, Vanhonacker and Frewer37). Exposure to the benefit-only message was found to result in an increased intention to eat fish (+21%), while the risk-only message translates into an 8% decrease in intention to eat fish(Reference Verbeke, Viaene, Frewer, Risvik and Schifferstein38, Reference Verbeke and Ward39). Balanced messages that include both risks and benefits do not significantly change intention to consume fish. However, other research has shown that negative information has more impact than positive information(Reference Frewer, Howard and Hedderley29, Reference Verbeke, Viaene, Frewer, Risvik and Schifferstein38Reference Richey, McClelland and Shimkunas41) and that consumers value information that has potential negative health effects more than information that conveys positive health effects(Reference Verbeke42).

Choosing the best medium for communication

In relation to the use of appropriate channels for communication, risk communicators normally rely on their public relations, advertising and media-buying partners for advice on targeting specific audiences. However, these data are rarely captured in the scientific literature. A useful review of elements of successful mass-media campaigns for behaviour change, including use of appropriate channels, has been published(Reference Noar43). Evaluation studies indicate that individual or small-group settings, such as information exchanges and public workshops, are the most effective venue for communicating trust factors(Reference Covello, Matthes, Bernhardt and Repacholi21, Reference Fischhoff, Covello, McCallum and Pavlova44). The effectiveness of written (i.e. brochure) v. computerised communication has been tested and no significant differences in learning outcome were found(Reference Fisher, King and Epp45, Reference Fisher46). Thus, at this stage it is not known whether there is an optimal mode for communicating risk.

The role of the media in risk communication

The media influence on risk perception is also still very much under debate. Media exposure would seem a logical influence of risk perceptions, especially given the vivid language, narratives and imagery often accompanying news stories. For example, the terminology ‘mad cow’ evoked high emotion. However, it is difficult to measure the social amplification of risks, i.e. why hazards or risk events with minor physical consequences frequently elicit strong public response and result in extremely severe social impacts.

There is widespread perception that media reporting is biased; for example, in a review of UK and Swedish media reporting it was found that reports tend to be negative and use alarmist headlines rather than reassuring ones(Reference Rowe, Frewer and Sjoberg47). However, other reviews have suggested that reporting of food risks is more neutral or moderate(Reference Wahlberg and Sjoberg31). Media reporting of risk may or may not provide the kind of information, e.g. statistics, that would allow an individual to assess their own risk. However, it has been suggested that the media may have an influence on the public's risk perception because the media express themselves in a way that the public can understand(Reference Wahlberg and Sjoberg31). In relation to media coverage of GM foods it has also been shown that changes in the volume and content of risk reporting can alter attitudes(Reference Frewer, Miles and Marsh48).

The growing reach and sophistication of digital communication is largely unexplored in relation to food risk communication. Certain subsets of the population now favour the internet, and especially social networking sites, along with mobile phone technology, as their preferred media channels and attempts to communicate with them using conventional media channels may fail. The influence of the internet on the rapid global spread of information on a food risk has recently been reviewed(Reference Hoijer, Lidskog and Thornberg49) following the publication of a report on the level of contamination in farmed salmon in Science (Reference Hites, Foran and Carpenter50). The widespread publicity that followed had immediate negative implications for the farmed salmon industry. It has also been demonstrated from the European Food Safety Authority's monitoring of media reporting of semicarbazide in baby food in 2006 that global coverage is expanded by the internet, with the highest publicity outside the EU noted in the USA(Reference Gassin and Geest51).

Characteristics of the recipient of risk messages

Finally, understanding the characteristics of an individual that influence how he or she will receive and act on risk information presents perhaps the most complex challenge. Past experience, existing knowledge, attitudes and beliefs, socio-demographic factors, personality factors, psychological variables, self-esteem, perception of vulnerability and affect heuristics such as optimistic bias may all influence how risk messages are accepted and whether an individual is likely to change related risk behaviour. Thus, audience segmentation has become increasingly important when crafting risk messages.

Previous experience, existing knowledge, attitudes and beliefs

If an individual, or someone known to them, has experienced the outcome of a risk (e.g. food poisoning) this factor will clearly intensify its personal relevance(Reference Fessenden-Raden and Heath52). In contrast, the extent to which an individual is knowledgeable about the topic or familiar with it may result in overconfidence or complacency(Reference Drottz-Sjöberg53). Existing beliefs and attitudes have also proved to be important predictors of risk perception and to influence acceptance of risk messages; for example, in relation to GM foods previous attitudes has been shown to be the strongest predictor of variance in perceived risk (86–90%) and benefit (92–95%)(Reference Frewer, Scholderer and Bredahl54). Also, more extreme or well-crystallised attitudes could influence perceptions of the information source, causing mistrust rather than perceptions of the risk being communicated(Reference Frewer, Scholderer and Bredahl54). In short, if a message is not in line with what an individual already believes it is likely to be dismissed or neglected(Reference Drottz-Sjöberg53).

Socio-demographic factors

Socio-demographic factors such as age, gender, socio-economic status, religious beliefs and world views also appear to be important in risk perception. It has been shown that men, particularly white men, view risks to be smaller when compared with women(Reference Slovic, Bazerman, Messick, Tenbrunsel and Wade-Benzoni55). A similar observation has been reported more recently, with women who have children and are full-time home makers rating risks more highly(Reference Kuttschreuter56, Reference Dosman, Adamowicz and Hrudey57). Older individuals also perceive greater risk(Reference Dosman, Adamowicz and Hrudey57) and are more likely to avoid risk(Reference Kuttschreuter56). Results relating to education level are equivocal. Higher education levels have been found to lead to less risk aversion(Reference Dosman, Adamowicz and Hrudey57), while subjects with a higher education worry more, feel less confident about the effectiveness of measures to prevent themselves from falling ill, feel less able to take such measures and have a lower level of trust in the safety of food products(Reference Kuttschreuter56). Socio-economic differences may also be important. Subjects with higher incomes have been shown to have higher risk avoidance(Reference Dosman, Adamowicz and Hrudey57), while those with lower incomes being less likely to engage in the risk communications process, particularly non-nationals who may have language barriers.

Cultural theory proposes that ‘worldviews’ such as fatalism, individualism, hierarchicism and egalitarianism could explain some variation in risk perception, and this approach seems intuitively plausible(Reference Douglas58). Individualists fear factors that might interfere with their individual freedom, egalitarians tend to mistrust experts and authority, hierarchists accept risk as long as decisions about such risks are justified by government or experts, while fatalists are quite indifferent (they try not to know or worry about situations they think they cannot change). However, these factors have subsequently been found not to make a large contribution in risk perception(Reference Sjoberg59).

Personality

As yet, many personality factors have not been assessed in relation to the success of risk communication; for example, an inherent willingness to take risks, feeling of vulnerability or future orientation. Some research has been carried out in relation to self-esteem and self-efficacy. For individuals with high self-esteem the idea that they are engaging in unhealthy behaviour may be more unacceptable than it would be to those with low self-esteem and may evoke a defensive response and rejection of the message. Those individuals with low self-efficacy may fail to act if they feel that they can do little about a health risk(Reference Boney-McCoy, Gibbons and Gerrard60, Reference Smith, Gerrard and Gibbons61).

Psychological factors

From a psychological perspective, individuals utilise various mental coping strategies to process complex risk information(Reference Fischhoff, Lichtenstein and Slovic62Reference Slovic, Fischhoff and Lichtenstein64). These cognitive processes for making quick assessments are often known as heuristics. Some examples are availability, anchoring and confidence or optimistic bias. ‘Availability’ means that an individual may have experienced the effects of a risk, or been exposed to images in television for examples, which means that it is easy to imagine. ‘Anchoring’ refers to an initial known risk perception or estimate; like a first impression, it is hard to move an individual from this point of view. ‘Optimistic bias’, means that individuals tend to believe they are less likely than others to experience harm(Reference Weinstein65).

How optimistic bias applies to food has been examined in some recent studies, although not extensively(Reference Frewer, Shepherd and Sparks66Reference Redmond and Griffith68). The studies have shown that individuals tend to show self-favouring biases for behavioural risk factors instrumental in the occurrence of health problems; for example, one study has indicated that individuals claim to eat less high-fat foods compared with the average individual(Reference Sparks, Shepherd and Wieringa69). It is suggested that these findings may help explain why nutritional messages fail; they fail because while individuals may accept the general validity of these messages, they may also deem them applicable to other individuals who eat more of the ‘risky’ food than they do.

Risk communication and behaviour change

Given the variety of factors influencing all aspects of the chain of communication between the messenger and the receiver of the risk information, the question remains ‘can risk communication affect behaviour change’. To a greater or lesser extent, this question is posed by every behaviour-change intervention reported in the literature (for an extensive review, see Gerrard et al.(Reference Gerrard, Gibbons and Reis-Bergan70)). Certainly, there is evidence that campaigns that communicate health risk have been associated with behaviour change in relation to major public health and safety issues such as population smoking levels, seatbelt wearing, sexual health behaviour in gay populations following information campaigns about HIV and AIDS and reduction in dietary fat intakes in the USA. What is not known at this stage is whether targeting risk information based on risk-perception segmentation outlined in the present paper can increase the effectiveness of the messages.

Conclusion

The complexities in food risk perception and communication outlined here represent only some of the challenges facing risk researchers today. Understanding these issues is important for public health policy makers and risk communicators as well as for the food industry. If individuals do systematically misperceive food risks and this misperception has both health and economic consequences, then addressing this issue is key. Future research on food risk perception must look to frontier methods of social science to disentangle causal processes linking perception, communication and behaviour. This approach will ensure that future risk communications programmes are both evidence-based and effective.

Acknowledgements

This work is funded by a grant from the Department of Agriculture and Food in Ireland under the Food Institutional Research Measures programme. The authors declare no conflict of interest. The paper is based on the lecture presented by P. W. A. M. was primarily responsible for researching, writing and editing the paper. E. H. assisted with the literature search and wrote drafts of some sections of the paper. Both L. D. and P. W. reviewed, commented and assisted with editing the paper.

References

1. Romer, D & Jamieson, P (2001) Do adolescents appreciate the risks of smoking? Evidence from a national survey. J Adolesc Health 29, 1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Slovic, P (2000) What does it mean to know a cumulative risk? Adolescents' perceptions of short-term and long-term consequences of smoking. J Behav Decision Making 13, 259266.Google Scholar
3. Steptoe, A & Wardle, J (2001) Risk awareness and emotional well-being in students from Eastern Europe and Western Europe. Soc Sci Med Health Behav 53, 16211630.Google ScholarPubMed
4. Sutton, S (1998) How ordinary people in Great Britain perceive the health risks of smoking. J Epidemiol Community Health 52, 338339.Google ScholarPubMed
5. Weinstein, N, Slovic, P & Gibson, G (2004) Accuracy and optimism in smokers' beliefs about quitting. Nicotine Tobacco Res 6, Suppl. 3, S375S380.Google ScholarPubMed
6. Fischhoff, B, Slovic, P, Lichtenstein, S et al. (1978) How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sci 9, 127152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7. Green, JM, Draper, A, Dowler, E et al. (2005) Public understanding of food risks in four European countries: a qualitative study. Eur J Public Health 15, 523527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Covello, VT, Peters, RG, Wojtecki, JG et al. (2001) Risk communication, the West Nile virus epidemic, and bioterrorism: responding to the communication challenges posed by the intentional or unintentional release of a pathogen in an urban setting. J Urban Health 78, 382391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9. Johnson, BB (1999) Ethical issues in risk communication: Continuing the discussion. Risk Anal 19, 335348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. World Health Organization (2004) Sixth Futures Forum on Crisis Communication. Copenhagen, Denmark: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
11. Sandman, PM (1987) Explaining risk to non-experts. Emerg Preparedness Dig Oct–Dec issue, 2529.Google Scholar
12. Covello, VT, McCallum, DB & Pavlova, MT (editors) (1989) Effective Risk Communication: The Role and Responsibility of Government and Nongovernment Organizations. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13. Hance, BJ, Chess, C & Sandman, PM (1989) Setting a context for explaining risk. Risk Anal 9, 113117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Lundgren, R (1994) Risk Communication: A Handbook for Communicating Environmental, Safety, and Health Risks. Columbus, OH: Battelle Press.Google Scholar
15. Boholm, A (1998) Comparative studies of risk perception: a review of twenty years of research. J Risk Res 1, 135163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16. Sjoberg, L (2000) Perceived risk and tampering with nature. J Risk Res 3, 353367.Google Scholar
17. World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe (2007) The Challenge of Obesity in the WHO European Region and the Strategies for Response. Copenhagen, Denmark: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
18. Siegrist, M, Kellerb, C & Kiers, HA (2006) Lay people's perception of food hazards: Comparing aggregated data and individual data. Appetite 47, 324332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Scherer, C (1991) Strategies for communicating risks to the public. Food Technol 45, 110116.Google Scholar
20. Sandman, PM (1989) Hazard versus outrage in the public perception of risk. In Effective Risk Communication: The Role and Responsibility of Government and Nongovernment Organizations, pp. 4549 [Covello, VT, McCallum, DB and Pavlova, MT, editors]. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21. Covello, V (1999) Risk perception, risk communication, and EMF exposure: Tools and techniques for communicating risk information. In Risk Perception Risk Communication, and Its Application to EMF Exposure: World Health Organization/ICNRP International Conference (ICNIRP 5/98), pp. 179214 [Matthes, R, Bernhardt, JH and Repacholi, MH, editors]. Vienna, Austria: International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection.Google Scholar
22. Fischhoff, B (1995) Risk perception and communication unplugged: Twenty years of progress. Risk Anal 15, 137145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23. Frewer, L (2004) The public and effective risk communication. Toxicol Lett 149, 391397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Siegrist, M & Cvetkovich, G (1999) Perception of hazards: The role of social trust and knowledge. Risk Anal 20, 713720.Google Scholar
25. Verbeke, W, Viane, J & Guiot, O (1999) Health communication and consumer behaviour on meat in Belgium: From BSE until dioxin. J Health Commun 4, 345357.Google ScholarPubMed
26. Slovic, P (1999) Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Anal 19, 689701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Chess, C, Salomone, KL, Hance, BJ et al. (1995) Results of a national symposium on risk communication: Next steps for government agencies. Risk Anal 15, 115125.Google Scholar
28. Frewer, LJ, Miles, S, Brennan, M et al. (2002) Public preferences for informed choice under conditions of risk uncertainty: the need for effective risk communication. Public Underst Sci 11, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29. Frewer, LJ, Howard, C, Hedderley, D et al. (1999) Reactions to information about genetic engineering: Impact of source characteristics, perceived personal relevance, and persuasiveness. Public Underst Sci 8, 3550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30. Lord Phillips of Worth Matravers, Bridgeman, J & Ferguson-Smith, M (2000) The BSE Inquiry: The Report. The Inquiry into BSE and CJD in the United Kingdom. London: The Stationery Office.Google Scholar
31. Wahlberg, A & Sjoberg, L (2000) Risk perception and the media. J Risk Res 3, 3150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32. Wynne, B (1989) Sheep farming after Chernobyl: A case study in communicating scientific information. Environment 31, 1039.Google Scholar
33. Wynne, B (1996) May the sheep safely graze? A reflexive view of the expert-lay knowledge divide. In Risk Environment and Modernity: Towards a New Ecology, pp. 4483 [Lash, S, Szerszynski, B and Wynne, B, editors]. London: Sage.Google Scholar
34. Theil, M (2002) The role of translations of verbal into numerical probability expressions in risk management: a meta-analysis. J Risk Res 5, 177186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35. Bier, VM (2001) On the state of the art: risk communication to the public. Reliab Eng Syst Safety 71, 139150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36. Johnson, FR, Fisher, A, Smith, VK et al. (1988) Informed choice or regulated risk? Lessons from a study in radon risk communication. Environment 30, 1235.Google Scholar
37. Verbeke, W, Vanhonacker, F, Frewer, LJ et al. (2008) Communicating risks and benefits from fish consumption: Impact on Belgian consumers' perception and intention to eat fish. Risk Anal 28, 951967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. Verbeke, W & Viaene, J (2001) Effects of communication (advertising or news) on sales of commodities. In Food People and Society: A European Perspective of Consumers' Food Choices, pp. 299315 [Frewer, L, Risvik, E and Schifferstein, H, editors]. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39. Verbeke, W & Ward, RW (2001) A freshmeat almost ideal demand system incorporating negative TV press and advertising impact. Agric Econ 25, 359374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
40. Chang, HS & Kinnucan, HW (1991) Advertising, information, and product quality – The case of butter. Am J Agric Econ 73, 11951203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41. Richey, M, McClelland, L & Shimkunas, A (1967) Relative influence of positive and negative information in impression formation and persistence. J Pers Soc Psychol 6, 322327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42. Verbeke, W (2005) Agriculture and the food industry in the information age. Eur Rev Agric Econ 32, 347368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43. Noar, SM (2006) A 10-year retrospective of research in health mass media campaigns: Where do we go from here? J Health Commun 11, 2142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
44. Fischhoff, B (1989) Helping the public make health risk decisions. In Effective Risk Communication: The Role and Responsibility of Government and Nongovernment Organizations, pp. 111116 [Covello, VT, McCallum, DB and Pavlova, MT, editors]. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
45. Fisher, AF, King, RN, Epp, DJ et al. (1994) Evaluating alternatives for communicating about food risk. J Appl Commun 78, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
46. Fisher, A (1992) Understanding Food Safety Policy Issues. Report on Model Materials. University Park, PA: Penn State Press.Google Scholar
47. Rowe, G, Frewer, L & Sjoberg, L (2000) Newspaper reporting of hazards in the UK and Sweden. Public Underst Sci 9, 5978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. Frewer, LJ, Miles, S & Marsh, R (2002) The media and genetically modified foods: Evidence in support of social amplification of risk. Risk Anal 22, 701711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49. Hoijer, B, Lidskog, R & Thornberg, L (2006) News media and food scares: the case of contaminated salmon. Environ Sci 3, 273288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
50. Hites, R, Foran, JA, Carpenter, DO et al. (2004) Global assessment of organic contaminants in farmed salmon. Science 303, 226229.Google ScholarPubMed
51. Gassin, AL & Geest, IV (2006) Communication in Europe on semicarbazide and baby food. J Risk Res 9, 823832.Google Scholar
52. Fessenden-Raden, J & Heath, JS (1987) Providing risk information in communities: Factors influencing what is heard and accepted. Sci Technol Human Values 12, 94101.Google Scholar
53. Drottz-Sjöberg, B-M (2003) Current Trends in Risk Communication. Theory and Practice. Oslo, Norway: Directorate for Civil Defence and Emergency Planning.Google Scholar
54. Frewer, LJ, Scholderer, J & Bredahl, L (2003) Communicating about the risks and benefits of genetically modified foods: The mediating role of trust. Risk Anal 23, 11171133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55. Slovic, P (1997) Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. In Environment, Ethics, and Behavior: The Psychology of Environmental Valuation and Degradation, pp. 277313 [Bazerman, MH, Messick, DM, Tenbrunsel, AE and Wade-Benzoni, KA, editors]. San Francisco, CA: The New Lexington Press.Google Scholar
56. Kuttschreuter, M (2006) Psychological determinants of reactions to food risk messages. Risk Anal 26, 10451056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57. Dosman, DM, Adamowicz, WL & Hrudey, SE (2001) Socioeconomic determinants of health- and food safety-related risk perceptions. Risk Anal 21, 307318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58. Douglas, M (1978) Cultural Bias. Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland Occasional Paper no. 35. London: Royal Anthropological Institute.Google Scholar
59. Sjoberg, L (2000) Factors in risk perception. Risk Anal 20, 112.Google ScholarPubMed
60. Boney-McCoy, B, Gibbons, FX & Gerrard, M (1999) Self-esteem, compensatory self-enhancement, and the consideration of health risk. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 25, 954965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
61. Smith, GE, Gerrard, M & Gibbons, FX (1997) Self-esteem and the relation between risk behavior and perceptions of vulnerability to unplanned pregnancy in college women. Health Psychol 16, 137146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62. Fischhoff, B, Lichtenstein, S, Slovic, P et al. (1981) Acceptable Risk. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
63. Slovic, P (1987) Perception of risk. Science 236, 280285.Google ScholarPubMed
64. Slovic, P, Fischhoff, B & Lichtenstein, S (1979) Rating the risks: the structure of expert and lay perceptions. Environment 21, 1439.Google Scholar
65. Weinstein, ND (1989) Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science 246, 12321233.Google ScholarPubMed
66. Frewer, LJ, Shepherd, R & Sparks, P (1994) The interrelationship between perceived knowledge, control and risk associated with a range of food-related hazards targeted at the individual, other people and society. J Food Safety 14, 1940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
67. Kearney, M, Gibney, MJ, Martinez, JA et al. (1997) Perceived need to alter eating habits among representative samples of adults from all member states of the European Union. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, S30S35.Google ScholarPubMed
68. Redmond, EC & Griffith, CJ (2004) Consumer perceptions of food safety risk, control and responsibility. Appetite 43, 309313.Google ScholarPubMed
69. Sparks, P, Shepherd, R, Wieringa, N et al. (1995) Perceived behavioural control, unrealistic optimism and dietary change: an exploratory study. Appetite 24, 243255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70. Gerrard, M, Gibbons, FX & Reis-Bergan, M (1999) The effect of risk communication on risk perceptions: the significance of individual differences. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr 25, 94100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Factors affecting risk communication.