Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T17:02:32.069Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's production of verb-phrase anaphora in a spoken task*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2008

MORAG L. DONALDSON*
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
LYNN S. M. COOPER
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
*
Address for correspondence: Dr M. L. Donaldson, School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ, UK. e-mail: Morag.Donaldson@ed.ac.uk

Abstract

To investigate the influence of semantic/pragmatic variables on children's production of verb-phrase anaphora (VPA), a spoken sentence completion task (e.g. John is throwing a ball and … Mary is too) was administered to four-, seven- and ten-year-olds. The frequency of VPA production was affected by whether the two clauses had the same or different polarity and by whether the actions were portrayed as simultaneous or sequential. These effects interacted in complex ways with age and with the presentation order of the polarity types. We speculate that developmental changes in the influence of semantic/pragmatic factors may be linked to increases with age in the strength of syntactic priming effects.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported in this paper was supported by a grant to both authors from the Economic and Social Research Council (reference number 000233191). We thank the children, parents and staff of Balgreen Nursery School and Balgreen Primary School, Edinburgh for their participation and cooperation. We are grateful to Holly Branigan, Peter Caryl, Robert Logie and Julia Simner for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

References

REFERENCES

Bamberg, M. (1987). The acquisition of narratives: Learning to use language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. & Capatides, J. B. (1987). Sources of meaning in the acquisition of complex syntax: The sample case of causality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 43, 112–28.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology 18, 355–87.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F. & Jones, M. (2005). A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children's noun phrases. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R. & Ha, S. (eds) Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University conference on language development, 109121. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P. & Duff, M. A. (1988). Young children's comprehension and production of causal expressions. Child Study Journal 18, 101119.Google Scholar
Clancy, P., Jacobsen, T. & Silva, M. (1976). The acquisition of conjunction: A cross-linguistic study. Stanford Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 12, 7180.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Hecht, B. F. (1983). Comprehension, production and language acquisition. Annual Review of Psychology 34, 325–49.Google Scholar
Diessel, H. (2004). The acquisition of complex sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. L. (1986). Children's explanations: A psycholinguistic study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. L. (1996). Contextual influences on children's spoken and written explanations. Applied Psycholinguistics 17, 355–75.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. L. & Laing, K. (1993). Children's comprehension and production of locative expressions. In Messer, D. & Turner, G. (eds) Critical influences on child language acquisition and development, 159–78. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Foley, C., Núñez del Prado, Z., Barbier, I. & Lust, B. (2003). Knowledge of variable binding in VP-ellipsis: Language acquisition research and theory converge. Syntax 6, 5283.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. & Clifton, C. Jr. (2005). The syntax–discourse divide: Processing ellipsis. Syntax 8, 121–74.Google Scholar
French, L. A. & Nelson, K. (1985). Young children's knowledge of relational terms: Some ifs, ors, and buts. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M. & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children's narratives: A comparison of English, French, German and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language 26, 419–52.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M. & Shimpi, P. (2004). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language 50, 182–95.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In Deutsch, W. (ed.) The child's construction of language, 121–47. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Levy, E. & Nelson, K. (1994). Words in discourse: A dialectical approach to the acquisition of meaning and use. Journal of Child Language 21, 367–89.Google Scholar
Li, P. & Shirai, Y. (2000). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. Volume 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Matsuo, A. & Duffield, N. (2001). VP-ellipsis and anaphora in child language acquisition. Language Acquisition 9, 301327.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (1985). A naturalistic study of the production of causal connectives by children. Journal of Child Language 12, 145–59.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 39, 633–51.Google Scholar
Postman, W., Foley, C., Pactovis, J., Rothenstein, B., Kaye, M., Lowe, D. & Lust, B. (1997). Children's knowledge of verbal inflection and LF raising: New evidence from elicited production of VP ellipsis structures. Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics 15, 3964.Google Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children's linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science 6, 557–67.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., de Villiers, J. & Hakuta, K. (1982). The development of sentence coordination. In Kuczaj, S. (ed.) Language development: Volume 1, syntax and semantics, 201244. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. & Wexler, K. (1999). Principle B, VP ellipsis, and interpretation in child grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar
Wigglesworth, G. (1990). Children's narrative acquisition: A study of some aspects of reference and anaphora. First Language 10, 105125.Google Scholar