Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-7qhmt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-26T21:14:59.925Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Feeling of Rationality: The Meaning of Neuroscientific Advances for Political Science

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 2004

Rose McDermott
Affiliation:
Rose McDermott is associate professor of political science at the University of California, Santa Barbara (rmcdermott@polsci.ucsb.edu)

Abstract

Recent advances in the neurosciences offer a wealth of new information about how the brain works, and how the body and mind interact. These findings offer important and surprising implications for work in political science. Specifically, emotion exerts an impact on political decisions in decisive and significant ways. While its importance in political science has frequently been either dismissed or ignored in favor of theories that privilege rational reasoning, emotion can provide an alternate basis for explaining and predicting political choice and action. In this article, I posit a view of decision making that rests on an integrated notion of emotional rationality.Rose McDermott is the author of Risk Taking in International Relations (1998) and Political Psychology in International Relations (2004) and works largely in the areas of political psychology, experimentation, and American foreign policy. The author is grateful to Jennifer Hochschild, Robert Jervis, and Stephen Rosen for generous and constructive advice and encouragement; and to Gerald Clore, Jonathan Cowden, Thomas Kozachek, Jonathan Mercer, Joanne Miller, Philip Zimbardo, the members of the Political Psychology and Behavior Workshop at Harvard, and anonymous reviewers for useful guidance and suggestions.

Passion is a sort of fever in the mind, which ever leaves us weaker than it found us.

—William Penn, Fruits of Solitude (1693)

We consider affective processing to be an evolutionary antecedent to more complex forms of information processing; but higher cognition requires the guidance provided by affective processing.

—Ralph Adolphs and Antonio Damasio, “The Interaction of Affect and Cognition” (2001)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 American Political Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alhakami, Ali, and Paul Slovic. 1994. A psychological study of the inverse relationship between perceived risk and perceived benefit. Risk Analysis 14 (6): 108596.Google Scholar
Adolphs, Ralph, and Antonio R. Damasio. 2001. The interaction of affect and cognition: A neurobiological approach. In Handbook of affect and social cognition, ed. Joseph P. Forgas, 2749. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Adolphs, Ralph, Daniel Tranel, and Antonio R. Damasio. 1994. Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 372 (6507): 66972.Google Scholar
Argyle, Michael. 1999. Causes and correlates of happiness. In Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology, ed. Daniel Kahneman, Ed Diener, and Norbert Schwartz, 35373. New York: Russell Sage.
Arnsten, Amy. 1998. The biology of being frazzled. Science 280 (5370): 171112.Google Scholar
Axelrod, Robert. 1984. The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.
Barber, James David. 1992. Presidential character: Predicting performance in the White House, 4th ed. New York: Prentice Hall.
Bechara, Antoine, Hanna Damasio, Daniel Tranel, and Antonio R. Damasio. 1997. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275 (5304): 129395.Google Scholar
Bechara, Antoine, Daniel Tranel, and Hanna Damasio. 2000. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123 (11): 21892202.Google Scholar
Bell, E. David. 1988. Disappointment in decision making under uncertainty. In Decision making: Descriptive, normative and prescriptive interactions, ed. David E. Bell, Howard Raiffa, and Amos Tversky, 35883. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bentham, Jeremy. 1982. An introduction to the principles of morals and legislation, ed. J. H. Burns and H. L. A. Hart. New York: Methuen. (Orig. pub. 1789.)
Bernoulli, Daniel. 1954. Exposition of a new theory on the measurement of risk. Econometrica 22 (1): 2336. (Orig. pub. 1738.)Google Scholar
Bernstein, Gary, and John Cacioppo. 2002. A contemporary perspective on multilevel analyses and social neuroscience. Unpublished manuscript.
Bower, Gordon H. 1981. Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36 (2): 12948.Google Scholar
Bower, Gordon H. 1983. Affect and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 302: 387402.Google Scholar
Bower, Gordon H. 1992. How might emotions affect learning? In The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory, ed. Sven-Åke Christianson, 331. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bower, Gordon H., and Joseph P. Forgas. 2001. Mood and social memory. In Handbook of affect and social cognition, ed. Joseph Forgas, 95120. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bueno de Mesquita, Bruce. 2000. Principles of international relations: People's power, preferences and perceptions. New York: CQ Press.
Campos, Joseph, and Karen Barrett. 1984. Toward a new understanding of emotions and their development. In Emotions, cognition and behavior, ed. Carol Izard, Jerome Kagan, and Robert Zajonc, 22963. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Clore, Gerald L. 1992. Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgment. In The construction of social judgments, ed. Leonard L. Martin and Abraham Tesser, 13363. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clore, Gerald L., and Andrew Ortony. 2000. Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes, or never? In Cognitive neuroscience of emotion, ed. Richard D. Lane and Lynn Nadel, 2461. New York: Oxford University Press.
Clore, Gerald L., Karen Gasper, and Erika Garvin. 2001. Affect as information. In Handbook of affect and social cognition, ed. Joseph P. Forgas, 12144. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clore, Gerald L., Robert Wyer, Bruce Dienes, Karen Gasper, Carol Gohm, and Linda M. Isbell. 2001. Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In Theories of mood and cognition, ed. Leonard L. Martin and Gerald L. Clore, 2762. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clore, Gerald L., and Linda M. Isbell. 2001. Emotion as virtue and vice. In Citizens and politics: Perspectives from political psychology, ed. James H. Kuklinski, 10323. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Damasio, Antonio R. 1996. Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam and Sons.
Damasio, Hanna, Thomas Grabowski, Randall Frank, Albert M. Galaburda, and Antonio R. Damasio. 1994. The return of Phineas Gage: The skull of a famous patient yields clues about the brain. Science 264 (5162): 11025.Google Scholar
Eich, Eric, and Dawn Macauley. 2000. Fundamental factors in mood dependent memory. In Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition, ed. Joseph P. Forgas, 10930. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Forgas, Joseph P. 1992. Affect in social judgments and decisions: A multi-process model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 25:22775.Google Scholar
Freud, Sigmund. 1923. Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. London: Allen and Unwin.
Fredrickson, Barbara. 2000. Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: The importance of peaks, ends and specific emotions. Cognition and Emotion 14 (4): 577606.Google Scholar
Gault, Barbara A., and John Sabini. 2000. The roles of empathy, anger and gender in predicting attitudes toward punitive, reparative, and preventive public policies. Cognition and Emotion 14 (4): 495520.Google Scholar
Gavanski, Igor, and Gary L. Wells. 1989. Counterfactual processing of normal and exceptional events. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 25 (4): 31425.Google Scholar
Gilbert, Daniel T., Elizabeth C. Pinel, Timothy D. Wilson, Stephen J. Blumberg, and Thalia P. Wheatley. 1998. Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75 (3): 61738.Google Scholar
Gilovich, Thomas, and Victoria H. Medvec. 1995. The experience of regret: What, why and when. Psychological Review 102 (2): 37995.Google Scholar
Goleman, Daniel. 1997. Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam.
Gray, Jeffrey A. 1987. The psychology of fear and stress. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Greenberg, Mark, and C. Kusche. 1998. Promoting alternative thinking strategies: PATHS, Blueprint for Violence Prevention, Book 10. Boulder, Colo.: Institute for Behavioral Sciences, University of Colorado.
Hertel, Guido, Jochen Neuhof, Thomas Theuer, and Norbert L. Kerr. 2000. Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? Cognition and Emotion 14 (4): 44172.Google Scholar
Hilgard, Ernest. 1980. The trilogy of mind: Cognition, affection, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 16 (2): 10717.Google Scholar
Hockey, G. Robert J., A. John Maule, Peter J. Clough, and Larissa Bdzola. 2000. Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition and Emotion 14 (6): 82356.Google Scholar
House, James S., Karl Landis, and Debra Umberson. 1988. Social relations and health. Science 241 (5865): 54045.Google Scholar
Inman, Jeffrey, and Marcel Zeelenberg. 2000. Would I rather fight than switch? Consumer regret following switch versus repeat decisions in outcome sequences. Unpublished manuscript.
Isen, Alice M. 1993. Positive affect and decision making. In Handbook of emotions, ed. Michael Lewis and Jeannette M. Haviland, 26177. New York: Guilford Press.
James, William. 1884. What is an emotion? Mind 9 (34): 188205.Google Scholar
Jervis, Robert. 1993. The drunkard's search. In Explorations in political psychology, ed. Shanto Iyengar and William J. McGuire, 33860. Durham: Duke University Press.
Johnson, Eric J., and Amos Tversky. 1983. Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45 (1): 2031.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel. 2000. Preface. In Choices, values and frames, ed. Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky, ixxvii. New York: Russell Sage Press.
Kahneman, Daniel, Ed Diener, and Norbert Schwartz. 1999. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Dale Miller. 1986. Norm theory: Comparing reality to alternatives. Psychological Review 93 (2): 13653.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47 (2): 26391.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1984. Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist 39 (4): 34150.Google Scholar
Keinan, Giora. 1987. Decision making under stress: Scanning of alternatives under controllable and uncontrollable circumstances. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (3): 63944.Google Scholar
Khong, Yuen Foong. 1992. Analogies at war: Korea, Munich, Dien Bien Phu, and the Vietnam decisions of 1965. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Lazarus, Richard S. 1982. Thoughts on the relation between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist 37 (9): 101924.Google Scholar
Lazarus, Richard S. 1984. On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist 39 (2): 12429.Google Scholar
LeDoux, Joseph E. 1995. Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology 46:20930.Google Scholar
Lerner, Jennifer S., and Dacher Keltner. 2000. Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion 14 (4): 47393.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, Peter, and Michael Rosenbaum. 1987. Recall of parental behavior by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and nondepressives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (3): 61119.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, George. 1996. Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making Processes 65 (3): 27292.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, George, and Jennifer Lerner. 2003. The role of affect in decision making. In The handbook of affective science, ed. Richard J. Davidson, Klaus R. Scherer, and H. Hill Goldsmith, 61942. New York: Oxford University Press.
Loewenstein, George, and Drazen Prelec. 1992. Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. In Choice over time, ed. George Loewenstein and Jon Elster, 11945. New York: Russell Sage.
Loewenstein, George, and Drazen Prelec 1993. Preferences for sequences of outcomes. Psychological Review 100 (1): 91108.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, George, and David Schkade. 1999. Wouldn't it be nice? Predicting future feelings. In Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology, ed. Daniel Kahneman, Ed Diener, and Norbert Schwartz, 85105. New York: Russell Sage.
Loomes, Graham, and Robert Sugden. 1982. Regret theory: An alternative of rational choice under uncertainty. Economic Journal 92 (386): 80524.Google Scholar
March, James. 1978. Bounded rationality, ambiguity, and the engineering of choice. Bell Journal of Economics 9 (2): 587608.Google Scholar
Marcus, George E., W. Russell Neuman, and Michael MacKuen. 2000. Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Marcus, George E., and Wendy M. Rahn. 1990. Emotions and democratic politics. Research in Micropolitics 3:2958.Google Scholar
Markman, Keith, Igor Gavanski, Stephen Sherman, and Matthew McMullen. 1995. The impact of perceived control on the imagination of better and worse possible worlds. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 (6): 58895.Google Scholar
Maslow, Abraham. 1970. Motivation and personality. Rev. ed. New York: Harper and Row.
Masters, Roger D. 1991. Individual and cultural differences in response to leader's nonverbal displays. Journal of Social Issues 47 (3): 15165.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose, and Jonathan Cowden. 2003. Hostile communications in a crisis simulation game. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Philadelphia.
Mellers, Barbara A. 2000. Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin 126 (6): 91024.Google Scholar
Mellers, Barbara A., Alan Schwartz, Katty Ho, and Ilana Ritov. 1997. Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science 8 (6): 42329.Google Scholar
Ritov, Ilana, and Jonathan Baron. 1990. Reluctance to vaccinate: Omission bias and ambiguity. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 3 (4): 26377.Google Scholar
Schkade, David, and Daniel Kahneman. 1998. Does living in California make people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction. Psychological Science 9 (5): 34046.Google Scholar
Schwarz, Norbert. 2000. Emotion, cognition and decision making. Cognition and Emotion 14 (4): 43340.Google Scholar
Schwarz, Norbert, and Gerald L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45 (3): 51323.Google Scholar
Simonson, Itamar. 1992. The influence of anticipating regret and responsibility on purchase decisions. Journal of Consumer Research 19 (1): 114.Google Scholar
Slovic, Paul. 1999. Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Analysis 19 (4): 689701.Google Scholar
Somit, Albert, and Steven A. Peterson. 1999. Rational choice and biopolitics: A (Darwinian) tale of two theories. PS: Political Science and Politics 32 (1): 3944.Google Scholar
Sullivan, Dennis G., and Roger D. Masters. 1988. “Happy warriors”: Leaders' facial displays, viewers' emotions, and political support. American Journal of Political Science 32 (2): 34568.Google Scholar
Taylor, Shelley E. 2002. The tending instinct: women, men, and the biology of relationships. New York: Henry Holt.
Tetlock, Philip E., and Richard Boettger. 1994. Accountability amplifies the status quo effect when change creates victims. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 7 (1): 123.Google Scholar
Thaler, Richard. 1999. Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 12 (3): 183206.Google Scholar
Vogel, Gretchen. 1997. Scientists probe feelings behind decision making. Science 275 (5304): 1269.Google Scholar
Von Neumann, John, and Oskar Morgenstern. 1947. Theories of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Way, Baldwin, and Roger Masters. 1996. Political attitudes: Interactions of cognition and affect. Motivation and Emotion 20 (3): 20536.Google Scholar
Zajonc, Robert B. 1980. Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist 35 (2): 15175.Google Scholar
Zajonc, Robert B. 1984. On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist 39 (2): 11723.Google Scholar
Zeelenberg, Marcel, and Jane Beattie. 1996. Consequences of regret aversion 2: Additional evidence for the effects of feedback on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making Processes 72 (1): 6378.Google Scholar
Zeelenberg, Marcel, Wilco van Dijk, Joop van der Pligt, Anthony Manstead, Pepijn van Empelen, and Dimitri Reinderman. 1998. Emotional reactions to the outcomes of decisions: The role of counterfactual thought in the experience of regret and disappointment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making Processes 75 (2): 11741.Google Scholar