Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T11:34:15.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of language of instruction on reasoning in bilingual children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Marguerite E. Malakoff*
Affiliation:
Yale University
*
Marguerite E. Malakoff, Yale University, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 11A Yale Station, New Haven, CT 06520-7447, U.S.A.

Abstract

The effect of language of instruction on verbal reasoning of balanced bilingual children was investigated. Subjects were 36 sixth-grade French-English bilinguals who were receiving primary instruction in either French or English. Eight types of verbal analogies were created by varying the language of the analogy stem (French or English), the language of the analogy solution (French or English), and level of difficulty (easy or hard). The primary analysis was conducted through ANOVA with program type as between-subjects variable and item types as repeated measures factors. The results showed a significant interaction between language of instruction and language of presentation. In addition, there were different language effects for the two groups. The introduction of the noninstructional language (French) had a significant effect on the mean response time of the English Language Program Group at both levels of difficulty. The French Language Program Group showed a more complex pattern of results: there was a statistically significant advantage for the All-English items as compared with the All-French items on the easy analogies but a trend towards poorer performance upon introduction of English on the hard analogies. These results are discussed in terms of the differences between the two groups of subjects and the effect of academic experience on language-related thought processes and fluency. The distinction between contextualized and decontextualized language is also discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Balkan, L. (1970). Les effets du bilinguisme français-anglais sur les aptitudes intellectuelles. [The effects of French-English bilingualism on intellectual aptitude]. Bruxelles: Almev.Google Scholar
Ben-Zeev, S.(1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 10091018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1984). Influences of bilingualism on metalinguistic development. In Language awareness/reading development: Cause? Effect? Concomitance? Symposium conducted at the National Reading Conference Meeting,St. Petersburg,Florida.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B. (in press). A metacognitive framework for the development of first and second language skills. In Forrest-Pressley, B. L., MacKinnan, G. E., & Waller, T. G. (Eds.), Meta-cognition, cognition, and human performance. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). Toward a definition of metalinguistic skill. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 229251.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1977). Cognitive factors associated with the attainment of intermediate levels of bilingual skill. Modern Language Journal, 61, 312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1978). Educational implications of mother tongue maintenance in minority-language groups. Canadian Modern Language Review, 34, 395417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1980). The cross-lingual dimensions of language proficiency: Implications for bilingual education and the optimal age issue. TESOL Quarterly, 14, 175187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1982). Linguistic interdependence among Japanese and Vietnamese immigrant students. In Rivera, C.(Ed.), The Measurement of Communicative Proficiency: Models and applications. Washington DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Cummins, J., & Gulutsan, M. (1974). Bilingual education and cognition. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 20, 259269.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. G., Kline, S. P., & Snow, C. E. (1986). Definitions and definite noun phrases: Indicators of children's decontextualized language skills. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 1, 3748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, R. (1983). Thought and two languages: The impact of bilingualism on cognitive development. In Gordon, E.(Ed.), Review of Research in Education. Washington DC: American Educational Research Association.Google Scholar
Diaz, R. (1985). Bilingual cognitive development: Addressing three gaps in current research. Child Development, 56, 13761388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duran, R. P., & Enright, M. (1983). Reading comprehension proficiency, cognitive processing mechanisms, and deductive reasoning in bilinguals. Final Report. Washington DC: National Institute of Education.Google Scholar
Galambos, S., & Hakuta, K. (1985). Does bilingualism affect metalinguistic performance? Paper presented at the American Association for Applied Linguistics, Baltimore, MD, 12 27–30, 1984.Google Scholar
Genesee, F. (1979). Scholastic effects of French immersion: An overview after ten years. Interchange, 9, 2029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. R., Reyes, M. R., & Vernhagen, C. K. (1984). Understanding fables in first and second languages. NABE Journal, 8, 3566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1982). Life with two languages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language: The debate on bilingualism. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K., & Diaz, R. (1985). The relationship between degree of bilingualism and cognitive ability: A critical discussion and some new longitudinal data. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's Language, Vol. 5. (pp. 320341). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K., Ferdman, B., & Diaz, R. (in press). Bilingualism and cognitive development: Three perspectives. In Rosenberg, S.(Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics, Vol. III: Reading, writing, and language learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ianco-Worrall, A. (1972). Bilingualism and cognitive development. Child Development, 43, 13901400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, W. E. (1975). Culture and language as factors in learning and education. In Wolfgang, A.(Ed.), Education of immigrant students. Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.Google Scholar
Lambert, W. E., & Tucker, G. R. (1972). Bilingual education of children: The St. Lambert experiment. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Mackay, W. F. (1972). Bilingual education in a bilingual school. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1966). Bilingualism and primary education. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1978). Second language acquisition in childhood. Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs, 76, (27, Whole No. 546).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skutnabb-Kangas, T., & Toukomaa, P. (1976). Teaching migrant children's mother tongue and learning the language of the host country in the context of the socio-cultural situation of the migrant family. Helsinki: The Finnish National Commission for UNESCO.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1985). Beyond conversation: Second language learner's acquisition of description and explanation. In Lantolf, J. & DiPietro, R.(Eds.), Second language acquisition in the classroom setting. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stemberg, R. J., & Nigro, G. (1980). Developmental patterns in the solution of verbal analogies. Child Development, 51, 2738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, R. (1972). Le Bilinguisme précoce. [Early bilingualism]. Bruxelles: Charles Dessar.Google Scholar