Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T21:09:55.525Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reexamining the Critical Period Hypothesis

A Case Study of Successful Adult SLA in a Naturalistic Environment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2008

Georgette Ioup*
Affiliation:
University of New Orleans
Elizabeth Boustagui
Affiliation:
Arab Maritime Transport Academy
Manal El Tigi
Affiliation:
American University in Cairo
Martha Moselle
Affiliation:
American University in Cairo
*
Address correspondence to Georgette Ioup, Department of English, University of New Orleans, New Orleans, LA 70148.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

This study concerns the ability of adults to achieve nativelike competence in second language when the acquisition context lacks formal instruction and, therefore, more closely resembles the environment for first language acquisition. The study presents the results of extensive testing of an adult who has apparently acquired native proficiency in Egyptian Arabic (EA) in an untutored setting. The goal is to determine to what extent her linguistic competence matches that of native speakers. Measures employed to assess her level of achievement are a speech production task, a grammaticality judgment task, a translation task, an anaphoric interpretation task, and an accent recognition task. Results are compared to those of native speakers as well as to those of a proficient learner of EA with extensive formal instruction. The results lead the authors to reexamine the critical period hypothesis while addressing the role of talent in adult language learning. The study concludes with an evaluation of our subject's language learning history to discover what factors differentiate her from less successful naturalistic adult acquirers.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

References

Birdsong, D. (1992). Ultimate attainment in second language acquisition. Language, 68, 706755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1990). The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis, 20, 349.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1965). The prediction of success in foreign language training. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), Training, research and education (pp. 87136). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1991). Cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude. In Perry, T. & Stansfield, C. (Eds.), Language aptitude reconsidered (pp. 1127). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Regents.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1990). The comparative study of first and second language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 135154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppieters, R. (1987). Competence differences between natives and fluent non-native speakers. Language, 63, 544573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farghaly, A. (1982). Subject pronoun deletion rule in Egyptian Arabic. In Gamal, S. & Bowers, R. (Eds.), Discourse analysis: Theory and application. Proceedings of the Second National Symposium on Linguistics and English Language Training (pp. 6069). Cairo: Center for Developing English Language Teaching, Ain Shams University.Google Scholar
Felix, S. (1985). More evidence on competing cognitive systems. Second Language Research, 1, 4772.Google Scholar
Flege, J. (1987). A critical period for learning to pronounce foreign languages? Applied Linguistics, 8, 162177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S. (1989). The role of the head-initial/head-final parameter in the acquisition of English relative clauses by adult Spanish and Japanese speakers. In Gass, S. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 306326). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, S., & Manuel, S. (1991). Age-dependent effects in language acquisition: ‘Critical period’ hypotheses. In Eubank, L. (Ed.), Point-counterpoint: Universal Grammar in second language (pp. 117145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F. (1988). Neuropsychology and second language acquisition. In Beebe, L. (Ed.), Issues in second language acquisition: Multiple perspectives (pp. 79112). Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Higgs, T., & Clifford, R. (1982). The push toward communication. In Higgs, T. (Ed.), Curriculum, competence, and the foreign language teacher (pp. 5779). Skokie, IL: National Textbook.Google Scholar
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of ESL. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Long, M. (1991). An introduction to second language acquisition research. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (1987). Instructed interlanguage development. In Beebe, L. (Ed.), Issues in second language acquisition (pp. 115141). New York: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neufeld, G. (1978). On the acquisition of prosodic and articulatory features in adult language learning. Canadian Modern Language Review, 34, 163174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neufeld, G. (1979). Towards a theory of language learning ability. Language Learning, 29, 227241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novoa, L., Fein, D., & Obler, L. (1988). Talent in foreign languages: A case study. In Obler, L. & Fein, D. (Eds.), The exceptional brain: The neuropsychology of talent and special abilities (pp. 294302). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Obler, L. (1989). Exceptional second language learners. In Gass, S., Madden, C., Preston, D., & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Variation in second language acquisition: Psycholinguistic issues (pp. 141159). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual matters.Google Scholar
Osman, M. (1990). The syntax and logical form of wh-interrogatives in Cairene Egyptian Arabic. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1976). A sensitive period in the acquisition of a non-native phonological system. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 261285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patkowski, M. (1980). The sensitive period for acquisition of syntax in a second language. Language Learning, 30, 449472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. New York: Atheneum Press.Google Scholar
Scarcella, R. (1983). Discourse accent in second language performance. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer and language learning (pp. 306326). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Schachter, J. (1989). Testing a proposed universal. In Gass, S. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 7388). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1983). Interaction, acculturation, and the acquisition of communicative competence: A case study of an adult. In Wolfson, N. & Judd, E. (Eds.), Sociolinguistics and language acquisition (pp. 137174). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneiderman, E., & Desmarais, C. (1988a). A neuropsychological substrate for talent in second language acquisition. In Obler, L. & Fein, D. (Eds.), The exceptional brain: Neuropsychology of talent and special abilities (pp. 103126). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, E., & Desmarais, C. (1988b). The talented language learner: Some preliminary findings. Second Language Research, 4, 91109.Google Scholar
Scovel, T. (1981). The recognition of foreign accents in English and its implications for psycholinguistic theories of language acquisition. In Scavard, J. & Laforge, L. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fifth Congress of AILA (pp. 389401). Laval, Canada: University of Laval Press.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1986). The role of foreign language aptitude in a model of school learning. Language Testing, 3, 188221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (1993). Cognitive abilities and language acquisition. Unpublished manuscript, Thames Valley University, London.Google Scholar
Smith, N., & Tsimpli, I. (1991). Linguistic modularity? A case study of a ‘Savant’ linguist. Lingua, 84, 315351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (1993). Incomplete vs. divergent representations of unaccusativity in non-native grammars of Italian. Second Language Research, 9, 2248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahba, W. (1984). Wh-constructions in Egyptian Arabic. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
White, L. (1989). The principle of adjacency in second language acquisition: Do L2 learners observe the subset principle? In Gass, S. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 134158). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1992). On triggering data in L2 acquisition: A reply to Schwartz and Gibala-Ryzak. Second Language Research, 8, 120137.Google Scholar
Yorio, C. (1989). Idiomaticity as an indicator of second language proficiency. In Hyltenstam, K. & Obler, L. (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 5572). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar