Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-hgkh8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T06:30:13.058Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: a cross-linguistic study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Soonja Choi*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
Alison Gopnik
Affiliation:
University of California at Berkeley
*
[*] Department of Linguistics, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182, USA. e-mail: soonja@mail.sdsu.edu.

Abstract

This cross-linguistic study investigates children's early lexical development in English and Korean, and compares caregivers' linguistic input in the two languages. In Study 1, the lexical development of nine Korean children was followed from 1;2 to 1;10 by monthly visits and maternal reports. These Korean data were compared to previously collected English longitudinal data. We find that: (1) Korean children as young as 1;3 use verbs productively with appropriate inflections. (2) Seven of the nine children show a verb spurt at around 1;7; for six of these children the verb spurt occurs before the noun spurt. No such early verb spurt is found in the English data. Unlike in English, both verbs and nouns in Korean are dominant categories from the single-word stage. (3) Korean children express language-specific distinctions of locative actions with verbs. Study 2, a crosslinguistic study of caregivers' input in English and Korean, shows that Korean mothers provide more action verbs but fewer object nouns than American mothers. Also, Korean mothers engage in activity-oriented discourse significantly more than American mothers. Our study suggests that verbs are accessible to children from the beginning, and that they may be acquired early in children who are encouraged to do so by their language-specific grammar and input.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Reilly, J. & Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child language 21, 85124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lifter, K. & Broughton, J. (1985). The convergence of early cognition and language in the second year of life: problems in conceptualization and measurement. In Barrett, M. D. (ed.), Children's single-word speech. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Tinker, E. & Margulis, C. (1992). The words children learn. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1978). The acquisition of word meaning: an investigation into some current conflicts. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Budwig, N. (1988). The linguistic marking of agentivity and control in child language. Journal of Child Language 16, 263–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. (1991). Early acquisition of epistemic meanings in Korean: a study of sentence-ending suffixes in the spontaneous speech of three children. First Language 11, 93119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41, 83121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clancy, P. (1986). The acquisition of communicative style in Japanese. In Schieffelin, B. & Ochs, E. (eds), Language acquisition across cultures. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Corrigan, R. (1978). Language development as related to stage 6 object permanence development. Journal of Child Language 5, 173–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, P., Bates, E., Reznick, J. S. & Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument of child language at 20 months. Journal of Child Language 16, 239–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dromi, E. (1987). Early lexical development. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. Child Development 64, 637–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development. Vol. 2: Language, thought one culture. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldfield, B. (1993). Noun bias in maternal speech to one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 20, 85100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldfield, B. & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language 17, 171–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A. (1982). Words and plans: early language and the development of intelligent actions. Journal of Child Language 9, 303–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1988). Three types of early words: the emergence of social words, names, and cognitive-relational words in the one-word stage and their relation to cognitive development. First Language 8, 4969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Choi, S. (1990). Do linguistic differences lead to cognitive differences? A crosslinguistic study of semantic and cognitive development. First Language 10, 199215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Choi, S. (In press). Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean-speakers: nouns are not always learned before verbs. In M. Tomasello & W. Merriman (eds), Relations between linguistic and cognitive development.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1985). Words, plan, things and locations: interactions between semantic and cognitive development in the one-word stage. In Kuczaj, S. & Barrett, M. (eds), The development of word meaning. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1986). Relations between semantic and cognitive development in the one-word stage: the specificity hypothesis. Child Development 57, 1040–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The development of categorization in the second year and its relation to other cognitive and linguistic developments. Child Development 58, 1523–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1993). Words and thoughts in infancy: the specificity hypothesis and the development of categorization and naming. In Rovee-Collier, C. & Lispitt, L. (eds), Advances in infancy research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., Pine, J. & Dresner Barnes, H. (1992). Individual differences in early vocabulary development: redefining the referential–expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 19, 287310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratsos, M. (1991). How the acquisition of nouns may be different from that of verbs. In Krasnegor, N., Rumbaugh, D., Schiefelbusch, R. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (eds), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Markman, E. (1987). How children constrain the possible meanings of words. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Markman, E. (1989). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science 14, 5777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McShane, J. (1980). Learning to talk. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language 17, 357–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38 (Serial Nos. 1–2), 1–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K., Hampson, J. & Kessler Shaw, L. (1993). Nouns in early lexicons: evidence, explanations and implications. Journal of Child Language 20, 6184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. (1983). Units of language acquisition. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A. & Sachs, J. (1990). Cognition and the verb lexicon in early lexical development. Applied Psycholinguistics 11, 409–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs. New York: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1984). Cognitive bases of lexical development: object permanence and relational words. Journal of Child Language 11, 477–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoh, M. H. (1982). Hankook atong-uy ene wheyktuk yenkwu [Language acquisition in Korean children]. Seoul: Seoul National University Press.Google Scholar