Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-xxrs7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T18:36:38.170Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verbal, visual, and spatial working memory demands during text composition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2008

THIERRY OLIVE*
Affiliation:
CNRS and University of Poitiers
RONALD T. KELLOGG
Affiliation:
Saint Louis University
ANNIE PIOLAT
Affiliation:
University of Provence
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Thierry Olive, Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l'Apprentissage—CNRS, Maison des Sciences de l'Homme et de la Société, 99 avenue du Recteur Pineau, F-86000 Poitiers, France. E-mail: Thierry.olive@univ-poitiers.fr

Abstract

Two experiments examined whether text composition engages verbal, visual, and spatial working memory to different degrees. In Experiment 1, undergraduate students composed by longhand a persuasive text while performing a verbal, visual, or spatial concurrent task that was presented visually. In Experiment 2, participants performed a verbal or spatial concurrent task that was aurally presented. Writing performance was not disrupted differentially across the three tasks. Performance on all concurrent tasks showed fewer correct responses and longer RTs relative to single-task, baseline data. However, the demands on visual working memory were as high as those on verbal working memory, whereas demands on spatial working memory were minimal. The findings help to delineate the roles of the verbal, visual, and spatial working memory in written composition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 28 287299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Bond, S. J., & Hayes, J. R. (1984). Cues peoples use to paragraph text. Research in the Teaching of English, 18 147167.Google Scholar
Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Peereman, R. (1998). Masked form priming in writing words from pictures: Evidence for direct retrieval of orthographic codes. Acta Psychologica, 99 311328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonin, P., Peereman, R., & Fayol, M. (2001). Do phonological codes constrain the selection of orthographic codes in written picture naming? Journal of Memory and Language, 45 688720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A. (1991). Issues in reading, writing, and speaking: A neuropsychological perspective. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Brain & Behavioral Sciences, 22 77126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chenoweth, N. A., & Hayes, J. R. (2003). The inner voice in writing. Written Communication, 20 99118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dédeyan, A., Olive, T., & Largy, P. (2006, October). Implication des composants de la mémoire de travail dans la détection des erreurs d'accord sujet–verbe: Approche développementale. Paper presented at the International Meeting Approche Cognitive de l'Apprentissage de la Langue Écrite, Rennes, France.Google Scholar
D'Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., Shin, R. K., Atlas, S., & Grossman, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Science, 378 279281.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, V. S., & Pashler, H. (2002). Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28 11871199.Google ScholarPubMed
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The dynamics of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In Gregg, L. W. & Steinberg, E. R. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3150). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Galbraith, D., Ford, S., Walker, G., & Ford, J. (2005). The contribution of different components of working memory to knowledge transformation during writing. L1 Educational Studies in Language and Literature, 5 113145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiglione, R., Landré, A., Bromberg, M., & Molette, P. (1998). L'analyse automatique des contenus [Automatic content analysis]. Paris: Dunod.Google Scholar
Haas, C., & Hayes, J. R. (1986). What did I just say? Reading problems in writing with the machine. Research in the Teaching of English, 20 2235.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. E. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 127). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R., & Grawdol-Nash, G. J. (1996). On the nature of planning in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. E. (Eds.), The science of writing. Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 2856), Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hecker, R., & Mapperson, B. (1997). Dissociation of visual and spatial processing in working memory. Neuropsychologia, 35 599603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99 122149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R. T. (1987). Effects of topic knowledge on the allocation of processing time and cognitive effort to writing processes. Memory & Cognition, 15 256266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. E. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 5771). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R.T. (1999). Components of working memory in writing. In Torrance, M. & Jeffery, G. (Eds.), The cognitive demands of writing: Processing capacity and working memory effects in text production (pp. 4361). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R. T. (2002). Competition for working memory among writing processes. American Journal of Psychology, 114 175191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellogg, R. T. (2004). Working memory components in written sentence generation. American Journal of Psychology, 117 341361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R. T., Olive, T., & Piolat, A. (2007). Verbal, visual and spatial working memory in written sentence production. Acta Psychologica, 124 382397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, A. (1992). The spatial coding hypothesis. In Rayner, K. (Ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition (pp. 379397). New York: Springer–Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, A., Brooks, R., Flynn, L. A., & Prophet, C. (2002). The reader's spatial code. In Hyönä, J., Radach, R., & Deubel, H. (Eds.), The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research (pp. 193212). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Largy, P., & Fayol, M. (2001). Oral cues improve subject–verb agreement in written French. International Journal of Psychology, 36 121132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, J., & Levy, M. C. (1999). Working memory as a resource in the writing process. In Torrance, M. & Jeffery, G. (Eds.), The cognitive demands of writing (pp. 6382). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Le Bigot, N., Passerault, J.-M., & Olive, T. (in press). Memory for words location in writing. Psychological Research.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1999). Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In Hagoort, P. & Brown, C. M. (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 94122). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, C. M., & Marek, P. (1999). Testing Kellogg's multicomponent model of working memory in writing: The role of the phonological loop. In Torrance, M. & Jeffery, G. (Eds.), Cognitive demands of writing (pp. 2541). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, C. M., & Ransdell, S. (2002). Writing with concurrent memory loads. In Olive, T. & Levy, C. M. (Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 930). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Logie, R. H., & Marchetti, C. (1991). Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial or central executive? In Logie, R. H. & Denis, M. (Eds.), Mental images in human cognition (pp. 105115). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovelace, E. A., & Southall, S. D. (1983). Memory for words in prose and their locations on the page. Memory & Cognition, 11 429434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madigan, R. J., Johnson, S. E., & Linton, P. W. (1994). Working memory capacity and the writing process. Paper presented at the American Psychological Society, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Marton, K., & Schwartz, R. G. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46 11381153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8 299325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCutchen, D. (2000). Knowledge acquisition, processing efficiency, and working memory: Implications for a theory of writing. Educational Psychologist, 35 1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miceli, G., Caltagirone, C., Capasso, R., Caramagno, A., Patria, F., Turriziani, P. et al. (2007). The neuroanatomy of orthographic working memory. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995). Examination of phonological working memory in specifically language-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16 355378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, S. T., Seymour, T. L., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2003). Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29 13531380.Google ScholarPubMed
Olive, T., Kellogg, R. T., & Piolat, A. (2002). The triple-task technique for studying the process of writing. In Olive, T. & Levy, C. M. (Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 3158). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olive, T., & Piolat, A. (2002). Suppressing visual feedback in written composition: Effects on processing demands and coordination of the writing processes. International Journal of Psychology, 37 209218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passerault, J.-M., & Dinet, J. (2000). The role of the visuo-spatial sketchpad in the written production of descriptive and argumentative texts. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 3 3142.Google Scholar
Piolat, A., Olive, T., Roussey, J.-Y., Thunin, O., & Ziegler, J. C. (1999). ScriptKell: A computer-assisted tool for measuring the relative distribution of time and cognitive effort in writing and other tasks. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 31 113121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piolat, A., Roussey, J.-Y. & Thunin, O. (1997). Effects of screen presentation on text reading and revising. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 47 565589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ransdell, S., Levy, C. M., & Kellogg, R. T. (2002). The structure of writing processes as revealed by concurrent task demands. L1 Educational Studies in Language and Literature, 2 141163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothkopf, E. Z. (1971). Incidental memory for location of information in text. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10 608613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sala, J. B., Räma, P., & Courtney, S. M. (2003). Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory. Neuropsychologia, 41 341356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shelton, J. R., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Deficits in lexical and semantic processing: Implications for models of normal language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6 528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychol-ogy, 33 542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., Kœppe, R. A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E. H., & Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial vs. object working memory: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7 337356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Plaats, R. E., & van Galen, G. P. (1990). Effects of spatial and motor demands in handwriting. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22 361385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed