Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T10:19:22.230Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonological memory and children's second language grammar learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2008

LEIF M. FRENCH*
Affiliation:
University of Quebec at Chicoutimi
IRENA O'BRIEN
Affiliation:
McGill University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Leif M. French, Applied Linguistics, Département des Arts et Lettres, Université du Québec à Chicoutimi, 555 Boulevard de l'Université, Chicoutimi, Québec G7H 2B1, Canada. E-mail: lfrench@uqac.ca

Abstract

This study examined the role of phonological memory in second language (L2) grammar learning in a group of native French-speaking children undergoing a 5-month intensive English program. Phonological memory (as referenced by Arabic [ANWR] and English [ENWR] nonword repetition tasks), L2 vocabulary (receptive and productive vocabulary knowledge), and L2 grammar (knowledge of morphosyntactic structures) were assessed during the first (Time 1) and last (Time 2) month of the program. After controlling for initial grammar ability, phonological memory significantly predicted grammar development (27.9% of variance explained) in addition to the contribution made by vocabulary knowledge (9.5% of variance explained). Although phonological memory ability as measured by ENWR increased between Time 1 and Time 2, ANWR did not improve. The findings show that phonological memory plays an important role in L2 grammar development that is unmediated by lexical knowledge. They also provide evidence that phonological memory improves with language development, but that basic phonological memory capacity (as measured by ANWR in this study) remains unchanged over time.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 403414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1996, 216233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 95116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkins, P. W. B., & Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 537552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 507584.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 2979). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blake, J., Austin, W., Cannon, M., Lisus, A., & Vaughan, A. (1994). The relationship between memory span and measures of imitative and spontaneous language complexity in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 91107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children's receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, R. D., Kurlan, M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Case, R. (1981). Syntactic form, semantic complexity, and short-term memory: Influences on children's acquisition of new linguistic structures. Developmental Psychology, 17, 367378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B., & Keijzer, R. (2000). What is hard to learn is easy to forget: The role of word concreteness, cognate status, and word frequency in foreign-language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Language Learning, 50, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufva, M., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1999). Native language literacy and phonological memory as prerequisites for learning English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 329348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 91126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (1998). Emergentism, connectionism and language learning. Language Learning, 48, 631664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Beaton, A. (1993). Factors affecting the learning of foreign language vocabulary: Imagery keyword mediators and phonological short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 533558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., Lee, M. W., & Reber, A. R. (1999). Phonological working memory in artificial language acquisition. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Sinclair, S. G. (1996). Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: Putting language in good order. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 234250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, L. M. (2006). Phonological working memory and L2 acquisition: a developmental study of Quebec francophone children learning English. New York: Edwin Mellen Press.Google Scholar
French, L. M., & O'Brien, I. (2007). Phonological memory and second-language speech production in children. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Gagnon, L., Kecskes, L., & Lipinski, K. (1998). Triple loop 4. Quebec: Modulo.Google Scholar
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 12131236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23, 8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E. (1998). The development of memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: the nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition. London: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Service, E., & Martin, A. J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (1999). Estimating the capacity of the phonological short-term store. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 378382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S. M. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 130.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (1987). Learning English in intensive programs in Quebec schools: 1986–1987. Report on the first year of research. Montreal: Concordia University.Google Scholar
Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (1994). An innovative program for primary ESL students in Quebec. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 563579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (1997). Learning English as a second language in a special school in Quebec. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 315355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 383388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). Contrasting contributions of phonological short-term memory and long-term knowledge to vocabulary learning in a foreign language. Memory, 13, 422429.Google Scholar
Munson, B. (2001). Phonological pattern frequency and speech production in adults and children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 778792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nimmo, L. M., & Roodenrys, S. (2002). Syllable frequency effects on phonological short-term memory tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 643659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical, narrative, and grammatical skills in second language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 377402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Freed, B., & Collentine, J. (2007). Phonological memory predicts second language oral fluency gains in adults. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 577582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottem, E. J., Lian, A., & Karlsen, P. J. (2007). Reasons for the growth of traditional memory span across age. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 233270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1986). Advanced Progressive Matrices: Set 1. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Roodenrys, S., & Hinton, M. (2002). Sublexical or lexical effects on serial recall of nonwords? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 2933.Google ScholarPubMed
Sasso, J., & Williams, W. (1998). Clues to English. Quebec: CEC Inc.Google Scholar
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 2150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speciale, G., Ellis, N. C., & Bywater, T. (2004). Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 293321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Freeman, D. J. (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorn, A. S. C., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 303324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Bon, W. H. J., & van der Pijl, J. M. L. (1997). Effects of word length and wordlikeness on pseudoword repetition by poor and normal readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 101114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Lely, H. J. K., & Gallon, N. (2006). Exploring the impact of higher linguistic representations on nonword repetition performance. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 591594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N., & Lovatt, P. (2003). Phonological memory and rule learning. Language Learning, 53, 67121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willis, C. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory contributions to sentence processing in young children. Memory, 9, 349363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed