Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-94d59 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T00:36:43.036Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Elicitation of the passé composé in French preschoolers with and without specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2008

PHAEDRA ROYLE*
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal, CHU Sainte-Justine, and CRLMB
ELIN T. THORDARDOTTIR
Affiliation:
McGill University and CRLMB
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Phaedra Royle, Université de Montréal, Faculté de Médecine, École d'orthophonie et d'audiologie, C.P. 6128, Succursale Centre-Ville, Montréal, Québec H3C 3J7, Canada. E-mail: phaedra.royle@umontreal.ca

Abstract

This study examines inflectional abilities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment (SLI) using a verb elicitation task. Eleven children with SLI and age-matched controls (37–52 months) participated in the experiment. We elicited the passé composé using eight regular and eight irregular high frequency verbs matched for age of acquisition. Children with SLI showed dissimilar productive verb inflection abilities to control children (even when comparing participants with similar verb vocabularies and mean length of utterance in words). Control children showed evidence of overregularization and sensitivity to morphological structure, whereas no such effects were observed in the SLI group. Error patterns observed in the SLI group demonstrate that, at this age, they cannot produce passé composé forms in elicitation tasks, even though some participants used them spontaneously. Either context by itself might therefore be insufficient to fully evaluate productive linguistic abilities in children with SLI.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bassano, D. (1998). Sémantique et syntaxe dans l'acquisition des classes de mots: L'exemple des noms et des verbs en français. Langue Française, 118, 2648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassano, D., Maillochon, I., Klampfer, S., & Dressler, W. U. (2001). L'acquisition de la morphologie verbale en français et en allemand autrichien: II. L'épreuve des faits. Enfance, 53 117148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., & Godham, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 507584.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 44, 905924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouffard, J., & Royle, P. (2007) La flexon des adjectifs: Étude de cas. Journée de la recherche des stagiaires de 1er cycle à la Faculté de médecine [Research colloquium for undergraduate trainees at the Faculty of Medicine]. Montréal: Université de Montréal.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Almazan, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franck, J., Cronel-Ohayon, S., Chillier, L., Frauenfelder, U. H., Hamann, C., Rizzi, L., et al. (2004). Normal and pathological development of subject–verb agreement in speech production: A study on French children. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 147180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goad, H. (1998). Plurals in SLI: Prosodic deficit or morphological deficit? Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 7, 247284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M. (1999). Familial language impairment: More English evidence. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 51, 519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, M., Dalalakis, J., Fukuda, S. E., Fukuda, S. & Kehayia, E. (1997). Genetic language impairment: Unruly grammars. In Runciman, W. G. & Maynard, J. (Eds.), Evolution of social behavior patterns in primates and man: 88. Proceedings of the British Academy (pp. 223249). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grégoire, A. (1937). L'Apprentissage du langage. Paris: Droz.Google Scholar
Hiriarteborde, A. (1973). Sur la généralisation de quelques marques grammaticales dans le langage d'enfants de 3 ans 1/2. Études de Linguistique Apliquée, 9, 101124.Google Scholar
Jakubovicz, C. (2003). Computational complexity and the acquisition of functional categories by French-speaking children with SLI. Linguistics, 41, 175211.Google Scholar
Jakubovicz, C., & Nash, L. (2001). Functional categories and syntactic operations in (ab) normal language acquisition. Brain and Language, 77, 321339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kilani-Schoch, M. (2003). Early verb inflection in French: An investigation of two corpora. In Bittner, D., Dressler, W. U., & Kilani-Schoch, M. (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kim, J. J., Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., & Coppola, M. (1994). Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure. Journal of Child Language, 21, 173209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legendre, G., Hagstrom, P., Vainikka, A., & Todorova, M. (2002). Partial constraint ordering in child French syntax. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 10, 189227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Bortolini, U., Caselli, M. C., McGregor, K. K. & Sabbadini, L. (1992). Morphological deficits in children with specific language impairment: The status of features in the underlying grammar. Language Acquisition, 2, 151179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Dromi, E., Adam, G., & Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S. (2000). Tense and finiteness in the speech of children with specific language impairment acquiring Hebrew. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 319335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Eyer, J., Bedore, L. & Grela, B. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 741753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21, 339366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G. F. (2001). The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (4, Serial No. 228).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. L'Année Psychologique, 101, 447462 (http://www.lexique.org)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2001). The morphosyntax of specific language impairment in French: An extended optional default account. Language Acquisition, 9, 269300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M., & Gopnik, M. (1997). Compensatory strategies in genetic dysphasia: Declarative memory. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 173185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Rice, M., Noll, K. R., & Grimm, H. (1997). An extended optional infinitive stage in German-speaking children with specific language impairment. Language Acquisition, 4, 255295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 850863.Google ScholarPubMed
Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, Y., & Royle, P. (1999). Uninflected structure in familial language impairment: Evidence from French. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 51, 7090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royle (2005–2007). Outils morphosyntaxiques pour le dépistage de la dysphasie chez les jeunes francophones [Morphosyntactic tools for the identification of SLI in young French speakers]. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Royle, P. (2007). Variable effects of morphology and frequency on inflection patterns in French preschoolers. The Mental Lexicon Journal, 2, 103125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royle, P., Jarema, G., & Kehayia, E. (2002a). Auditory verb recognition in developmental language impairment. Brain and Language, 81, 487500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royle, P., Jarema, G., & Kehayia, E. (2002b). Frequency effects on visual word access in developmental language impairment. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 1141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royle, P., Jarema, G., & Kehayia, E. (2003). Verb reading in developmental language impairment. Brain and Language, 87, 311322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royle, P., Rivard, M.-È., & Thordardottir, E. (2004). Perfect acquisition in French-speaking pre-schoolers. Paper presented at the 4th International Conference on the Mental Lexicon, Windsor, ON, Canada, June 31–July 4, 2004.Google Scholar
Royle, P., Vial, M., & Valois, D. (in press). The acquisition of concord in French and Spanish determiner phrases, two elicitation experiments. Revista de la Asociación Española de Lingüística Aplicada.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E., Gagné, A., Levy, L., Kehayia, E., Lessard, N., Sutton, A., et al. (2005) Spontaneous language sample measures for French-speaking 5-year-olds. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA), San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. T. (2005). Early lexical and syntactic development in Quebec French and English: Implications for cross-linguistic and bilingual assessment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 243278CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thordardottir, E. T., & Namazi, M. (2007). Specific language impairment in French-speaking children: Beyond grammatical morphology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 698715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trudeau, N., Frank, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Une adaptation en français du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. Revue d'Orthophonie et d'Audiologie, 23, 6173.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Stollwerck, L. (1996). A grammatical specific language impairment in children: An autosomal dominant inheritance? Brain & Language, 52, 484504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Ullman, M. T. (2001). Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Language & Cognitive Processes, 16, 177217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T., & Gopnik, M. (1999). Inflectional morphology in a family with inherited specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 51117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K. (1994). Optional infinitives, head movement, and economy of derivations. In Horenstein, N. & Lightfoot, D. (Eds.), Verb movement (pp. 305362). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K. (1998). Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 2379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar