Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T02:22:31.238Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Acquiring causatives in Taiwan Southern Min*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2008

HUEI-LING LIN*
Affiliation:
National Chung Cheng University
JANE S. TSAY*
Affiliation:
National Chung Cheng University
*
Address for correspondence: Department of Foreign Languages & Literature, National Chung Cheng University, Min-Hsiung, Chiayi 62107Taiwan, R.O.C.
Address for correspondence: Department of Foreign Languages & Literature, National Chung Cheng University, Min-Hsiung, Chiayi 62107Taiwan, R.O.C.

Abstract

This case study is based on the longitudinal data of a girl (LYC, 1 ; 2–3 ; 3) acquiring Taiwan Southern Min (TSM) as her first language, and it aims to discover the overgeneralization pattern of children acquiring causatives in TSM. Among the three types of causative, the errors found in other languages are mostly with lexical causatives; however, in TSM, the errors occur with morphological and analytic causatives. Being an analytic language, TSM tends to spell out the causative meaning through morphological and analytic causatives and thus most errors occur with these two types. In contrast, lexical causatives, which contain a semantic element CAUSE, were acquired late; in the data collected (1 ; 2–3 ; 3) lexical causatives were not yet found. This case study provides evidence from TSM to show a different overgeneralization pattern.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Research for this paper was supported by a grant from the National Science Council (NSC 93-2420-H-194-003-KD). We would like to thank Professor James H.-Y. Tai for his valuable comments on the draft of this paper.

References

Allen, S. E. M. (1998). Categories within the verb category: learning the causative in Inuktitut. Linguistics 36, 633–77.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1993). Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 20, 641–69.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1974). Learning the structure of causative verbs: a study in the relationship of cognitive, semantic and syntactic development. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 8, 142–78.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1988). Modeling the acquisition of linguistic structure. In Levy, Y., Schlesinger, I. M. & Braine, M. D. S. (eds) Categories and processes in language acquisition, 217–59. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S., Brody, R. E., Fisch, S. M., Weisberger, M. J. & Blum, M. (1990). Can children use a verb without exposure to its argument structure? Journal of Child Language 17, 313–42.Google Scholar
Cheng, R. L. (1974). Causative constructions in Taiwanese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 2, 279324.Google Scholar
Cheng, R. L. (1985). A comparison of Taiwanese, Taiwan Mandarin, and Peking Mandarin. Language 61, 352–77.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S., Huang, C.-T. J., Li, Y.-H. A. & Tang, C.-C. J. (1997). Causative compounds across Chinese dialects: a study of Cantonese, Mandarin and Taiwanese. In Tseng, C.-Y. (ed.) Chinese languages and linguistics IV: typological studies of languages in China, 199224. Taipei: Academia Sinica.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S., Huang, C.-T. J., Li, Y.-H. A. & Tang, C.-C. J. (1999). Hoo hoo hoo: syntax of the causative, dative, and passive constructions in Taiwanese. In Ting, P.-H. (ed.) Contemporary studies on the Min dialects, 146203. Journal of Chinese Linguistics Monograph Series #14.Google Scholar
Cheung, S. L. (1998). Causatives verbs in child Cantonese. In Clark, E. V. (ed.) The proceedings of the twenty-ninth annual child language research forum, 151–60. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (1981). Language universals and linguistic typology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Figueira, R. A. (1984). On the development of the expression of causativity: a syntactic hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 11, 109–27.Google Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J. (1991). Remarks on the status of the null objects. In Freidin, R. (ed.) Principles and parameters in comparative grammar, 5676. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lien, C. (1999). A typological study of causatives in Taiwanese Southern Min. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies 29, 395422.Google Scholar
Lin, H.-L. (2001). Verbal complexes and objects in Taiwanese. In Liou, H.-C. & Chung, R.-F. (eds) Language research and English teaching: challenges and solutions, 5574. Taipei: The Crane Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Lin, H.-L. (2006). Morphological causatives versus resultative compounds in Taiwan Southern Min. In Chung, R.-F., Liou, H.-C., Hsu, J.-L. & Ho, D.-A. (eds) On and off work: festschrift in honor of Professor Chin-Chuan Cheng on his 70th birthday, 113–26. Taipei: Institute of Linguistics, Academic Sinica.Google Scholar
Lord, C. (1979). “Don't you fall me down”: children's generalizations regarding cause and transitivity. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 17, 81–9.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pye, C. (1994). A cross-linguistic approach to the causative alternation. In Levy, Y. (ed.) Other children, other languages: issues in the theory of language acquisition, 243–63. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shibatani, M. & Pardeshi, P. (2001). The causative continuum. In Shibatani, M. (ed.) The grammar of causation and interpersonal manipulation, 85126. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Shirai, Y., Miyata, S., Naka, N. & Sakazaki, Y. (2000). The acquisition of causative morphology: why does it correlate with the imperative? In Clark, E. V. (ed.) The proceedings of the thirtieth annual child language research forum, 8794. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Tsay, J. S. (2005). Taiwan Child Language Corpus: data collection and annotation. Proceedings of 5th workshop on Asia language resources, 5661. Jeju Island, Republic of Korea.Google Scholar
Tsay, J. S. (in preparation). Taiwan Child Language Corpus. National Chung Cheng University, Taiwan.Google Scholar