Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T10:48:14.549Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Overextension and underextension in the child's expressive and receptive speech*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Deborah A. Kay
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo
Jeremy M. Anglin
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo

Abstract

The tendency of 30 children between the ages of 2; 0 and 2; 1 to overextend and/or underextend newly uttered, but previously understood words in comprehension and production was examined. It was found that both overextensions and underextensions occurred in comprehension and in production. Similarly, in both modes of speech children made more underextensions to peripheral than to central referents, and more overextensions to perceptually similar than to perceptually dissimilar nonreferents. The frequencies of overextensions and underextensions, however, were not the same in comprehension and production. Overextensions occurred more frequently in comprehension than in production, while conversely underextensions occurred more frequently in production than in comprehension. These data are discussed in terms of differences between children's and adult's word meanings, and between comprehension and production.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. M. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, A. F. & Chamberlain, J. C. (1904). Studies of a child. Pedagogical Seminary 11. 264–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E.. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1975). Knowledge, context, and strategy in the acquisition of meaning. In Dato, D. P.. (ed.), Georgetown University round table on languages and linguistics 1975. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978). Strategies for communicating. ChDev 49. 953–9.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1979). Building a vocabulary: words for objects, actions, and relations. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M.. (eds), Studies in language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and language New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Fremgen, A. & Fay, D. (1980). Overextensions in production and comprehension: a methodological clarification. JChLang 7. 205–11.Google ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. (1973). Who is ‘Dada’? Some aspects of the semantic and phonological development of a child's first words. L & S 16. 3443.Google ScholarPubMed
Gruendel, J. M. (1977). Referential extension in early language development. ChDev 48. 1567–76.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L..(ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology. Potomac: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1979). Young children's overextensions of object words in comprehension and/or production: support for a prototype theory of early word meaning. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,San Francisco.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1939). Speech development of a bilingual child: a linguistic record. Vol. 1, Vocabulary growth in the first two years. Evanston, Ill.: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1949). Speech development of a bilingual child. Vol. 3, Grammar and general problems in the first two years. Evanston, Ill.: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. M. (1959). How children learn to speak. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Moore, K. C. (1896). The mental development of the child. PsychRev.Monogr.Supplements 1.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr.Soc.Res.Ch.Devel. 149.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word and sentence: interrelations in acquisition and development. PsychRev 81. 267–85.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Rescorla, L., Gruendel, J. & Benedict, H. (1978). Early lexicons: what do they mean? ChDev 49. 960–8.Google Scholar
Reich, P. A. (1976). The early acquisition of word meaning. JChLang 3. 117–23.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. A. (1976). Concept formation in word learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Moore, T. E.. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Saltz, E. (1971). The cognitive bases of human learning. Homewood, Ill.: The Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
Saltz, E., Dixon, D., Klein, S. & Becker, G. (1977). Studies of natural language concepts. III. Concept overdiscrimination in comprehension between two and four years of age. ChDev 48. 1682–5.Google Scholar
Thomson, J. R. & Chapman, R. S. (1977). Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: the status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension. JChLang 4. 359–75.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar