Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ph5wq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T00:19:44.629Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effects of intonation on infant attention: the role of the rising intonation contour*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Joseph W. Sullivan
Affiliation:
University of Colorado School of Medicine
Frances Degen Horowitz
Affiliation:
University of Kansas

Abstract

The study was designed to investigate 2-month-old infant preferential attention to a feature found to be characteristic of mothers' speech to their infants. A modified infant-control auditory preference paradigm was employed to assess infants' differential attention to synthetically generated and naturally produced rising and falling intonation contours. Analysis of these data revealed that the infants attended more to the rising naturally produced intonation contour. A reverse pattern of greater attention to the falling contour was found with the synthetically generated stimuli. In addition, inspection of the results permitted the conclusion that the infant-control preference paradigm was a viable method for assessing the 2-month-old infant's preferential attention to auditory stimuli. The results are discussed in terms of their relevance to the study of the infant's developing language reception abilities.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, J. L. & Ramey, C. T. (1980). Structural aspects of maternal speech to infants reared in poverty. ChDev 55. 1280–4.Google Scholar
Brown, R. & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In Lenneberg, E. H. (ed.), New directions in the study of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. S. (1981). Mother-child interaction in the second year of life: its role in language development. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. &Bricker, D. (eds), Early language: acquisition and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Cherry, L. J. (1976). Interactive strategies in language development: model of social cognition. Paper presented to the Conference on Language, Children, and Society,Columbus, Ohio.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1973). Linguistic mythology and the first year of life. BJDisComm 8. 2936.Google ScholarPubMed
Crystal, D. (1976). Developmental intonology. In Rafiler-Engel, W. F. &Lebrun, Y. (eds), Baby talk and infant speech. Amsterdam: Swetz & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1978). The analysis of intonation in young children. In Minifie, F. D. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds). Communication and cognitive abilities: early behavioral assessment. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1979). Prosodic development. In Fletcher, P. &Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. A. (1978). Semantics and syntax in the first two years: the output of form and function and the form and function of input. In Minifie, F. D. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Communicative and cognitive abilities: early behavioral assessment. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D. & Tartter, V. C. (1979). On the development of speech perception: mechanisms and analogies. In Reese, H. W. & Lipsitt, L. P. (eds), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 13. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Emde, R. D., Gaensbauer, T. J. & Harmon, R. J. (1976). Emotional expression in infancy: a biobehavioral study. Psychollssues 10. Monogr. 37.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. AmAnth 66. 103–13.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1981). 4-month-olds prefer to listen to ‘motherese’. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, March.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Kuhl, P. (1981). Fundamental frequency as an acoustic determinant of infant preference for motherese. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, March.Google Scholar
Gillman, C. B. & Wilson, D. L. (1979). A unified system for the synthesis, presentation, and analysis of speech. Paper presented to the National Conference of the Use of On-Line Computers in Psychology, November.Google Scholar
Giliman, C., Wilson, D., Hirsch, J. & Morse, P. (1975). VOCAL: a computer-based system for preparing auditory stimuli. Infant Development Laboratory, University of Wisconsin-Madison. Research Status Report 1. 285–96.Google Scholar
Horowitz, F. D. (1980). Receptive language development in the first year of life: a selective review of literature. In Gallagher, J. J. (ed.), New directions for exceptional children. San Francisco: Jossey-Baas.Google Scholar
Horowitz, F. D., Paden, L. Y., Bhana, K. & Self, P. A. (1972). An infant control procedure for studying infant visual fixations. DevPsych 7. 90.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. & Thompson, E. (1978). Perception of a phonetic contrast in multisyllabic utterances by 2-month-old infants. PerceptPsychophys 23. 105–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E. (1969). The role of intonation in the acquisition of language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E. & Kaplan, G. (1971). The prelinguistic child. In Eliot, J. (ed), Human development and cognitive processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Kuhi, P. K. & Hillenbrand, J. (1979). Speech perception by young infants: perceptual constancy for categories based on pitch contour. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco, March.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. M. (1936). Infant speech. London: Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Mehier, J., Bertoncini, J., Barrière, M. & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D. (1978). Infant recognition of mother's voice. Percept 7. 491–7.Google Scholar
Morse, P. A. (1972). The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy. JExpChPsych 14. 477–92.Google ScholarPubMed
Newport, E. L. (1977). Motherese: the speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, N. J., Pisoni, D. B. & Potts, G. R. (eds), Cognitive theory, Vol. 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. The adaptive significance of linguistic input to prelinguistic infants. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mother's speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1979). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. JCh Lang 4. 122.Google Scholar
Snow, C., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J. & Vorster, J. (1976). Mother's speech in three social classes. JPsycholingRes 5. 520.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds). Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Spring, D. R. & Dale, P. S. (1977). Discrimination of linguistic stress in early infancy. JSpHearRes 20. 224–32.Google ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., Barnett, R. K. & MacKain, K. (1983). The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes. JChLang 10. 115.Google ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S. & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to pre-linguistic infants. DevPsych 18. 727–35.Google Scholar
Sullivan, J. W. & Horowitz, F. D. (1983). Infant intermodal perception and maternal multi-modal stimulation: implications for language development. In Lipsitt, L. P. & Royce-Collier, C. K. (eds), Advances in infancy research, Vol. 2. New Jersey: Ablex.Google Scholar
Tuaycharoen, P. (1978). The babbling of a Thai baby: echoes and responses to the sounds made by adults. In Waterson, N. &Snow, K. (eds), Development of communication: social and pragmatic factors in language acquisition. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winer, B. J. (1971). Data analysis: a statistical primer for psychology students. Chicago: Aldine & Atherton.Google Scholar