Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:26:11.872Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are subjective well-being measures any better than decision utility measures?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2008

DYLAN M. SMITH*
Affiliation:
VA Center for Practice Management and Outcomes Research, VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, Center for Behavioral and Decision Sciences in Medicine, University of Michigan School of Medicine and Department of Internal Medicine, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, USA
STEPHANIE L. BROWN
Affiliation:
VA Center for Practice Management and Outcomes Research, VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, Center for Behavioral and Decision Sciences in Medicine, University of Michigan School of Medicine and Department of Internal Medicine, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, USA
PETER A. UBEL
Affiliation:
VA Center for Practice Management and Outcomes Research, VA Ann Arbor Healthcare System, Ann Arbor, Center for Behavioral and Decision Sciences in Medicine, University of Michigan School of Medicine and Department of Internal Medicine, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Dylan M. Smith, Department of Internal Medicine, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA. Email: dylsmith@umich.edu

Abstract

There are a number of substantial problems with using decision-based utility measures such as the time trade off and standard gamble to value improvements in health. Dolan (this issue) argues that because of these problems, it would be better to use measures of real experiences (subjective well being). We review evidence that supports criticisms of decision-based utility measures, whether provided by patients or non-patients. But we also review a number of substantial problems with currently used measures of subjective well-being, and point out that there is no definitive evidence that they represent any improvement over decision utility measures. We conclude with a call for expanded research into developing new tools for quantifying health-related quality of life that are more valid, more sensitive to changes in health status, and less biased.

Type
Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albrecht, G. L. and Devlieger, P. J. (1999), ‘The disability paradox: high quality of life against all odds’, Social Science and Medicine, 48(8): 977988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brickman, P. et al. (1978), ‘Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative?’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(8): 917927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Wit, G. A. et al. (2000), ‘Sensitivity and perspective in the valuation of health status: whose values count?’, Health Economics, 9(2): 109126.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E. and Lucas, R. E. (1999), ‘Personality and subjective well-being’, in Kahneman, D.Diener, E. and Schwarz, N. (eds), Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Dolan, P. (2007), ‘Developing methods that really do value the “Q” in the QALY’, Health Economics, Policy and Law, 3(1).Google Scholar
Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J. et al. (1996), ‘Adrenal secretion during major depression in 8- to 16-year-olds, I: altered diurnal rhythms in salivary cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) at presentation’, Psychological Medicine, 26(2): 245256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. et al. (2004), ‘A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: the day reconstruction method’, Science, 306(5702): 17761780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S. (1991), Emotion and Adaptation, Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. and Schkade, D. (1999), ‘Wouldn't it be nice? Predicting future feelings’, in Kahneman, D., Diener, E. and Schwarz, N. (eds), Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Pavot, W. and Diener, E. (1993), ‘Review of the satisfaction with life scale’, Psychological Assessment, 5(2): 164172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin, M. J. et al. (in submission), ‘A helping hand versus a watchful eye: helping behavior and well-being among spouse caregivers’.Google Scholar
Rainer, J. (2000), ‘Compassion fatigue: when care giving begins to hurt’, Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book, Sarasota: Professional Resource Press.Google Scholar
Riis, J. et al. (2005), ‘Ignorance of hedonic adaptation to hemo-dialysis: a study using ecological momentary assessment’, Journal Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1): 39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, M. (1989), ‘Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories’, Psychological Review, 96(2): 341357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. and Strack, F. (1999), ‘Reports of subjective well-being: judgmental processes and their methodological implications’, in Kahneman, D., Diener, E. and Schwarz, N. (eds), Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Shiffman, S. and Stone, A. A. (1998), ‘Ecological momentary assessment: a new tool for behavior medicine research’, in Krants, D. S. (ed.), Technology and Methods in Behavioral Medicine, Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 117–131.Google Scholar
Smith, D. M. et al. (2006a), ‘Why are you calling me? How study introductions change response patterns’, Qual Life Res, 15(4): 621–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D. M. et al. (2006b), ‘Misremembering colostomies? Former patients give lower utility ratings than do current patients’, Health Psychol, 25(6): 688695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, D. M. et al. (in press), ‘Mispredictions and misrecollections: challenges for subjective outcome measurement’, Disability and Rehabilitation.Google Scholar
Smith, D. M. et al. (in submission), ‘Mispredicting and misremembering: patients overestimate improvements in quality of life after renal transplant’.Google Scholar
Ubel, P. A. et al. (2001), ‘Do nonpatients underestimate the quality of life associated with chronic health conditions because of a focusing illusion?’, Medical Decision Making, 21(3): 190199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ubel, P. A. et al. (2005a), ‘What is perfect health to an 85-year-old?: Evidence for scale recalibration in subjective health ratings’, Medical Care, 43(10): 10541057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ubel, P. A. et al. (2005b), ‘Misimagining the unimaginable: the disability paradox and healthcare decision making’, Health Psychology, 24(4 Supplement): S57S62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D. and Gilbert, D. T. (2003), ‘Affective forecasting’, in Zanna, M. P. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35: 345–341, San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar