Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T08:11:30.655Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sexual orientation related differences in spatial memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 February 2003

Rahman Qazi*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London, UK
Wilson Glenn D.
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London, UK
Abrahams Sharon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London, UK
*
Reprint requests to: Qazi Rahman, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London, De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. E-mail: q.rahman@iop.kcl.ac.uk

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate and extend previously reported sex differences in object location memory by comparing the performance of heterosexual and homosexual males and females. Subjects were 240 healthy, right-handed heterosexual and homosexual males and females. They were instructed to study 16 common, gender-neutral objects arranged randomly in an array and subsequently tested for object recall, object recognition and spatial location memory. Females recalled significantly more objects than males, although there were no group differences in object recognition. Decomposition of significant interactions between sex and sexual orientation on spatial location memory (controlling for differences in object recall, age and IQ) revealed that heterosexual females and homosexual males scored better than heterosexual males, and no different from each other. There were no differences between homosexual and heterosexual females. The findings suggest that homosexual males and heterosexual females encode, store and retrieve positional and relational information about spatial layouts similarly, pointing to within-sex variations in the neural architecture underlying spatial memory. (JINS, 2003, 9, 376–383.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahams, S., Morris, R.G., Polkey, C.E., Jarosz, T.C.S., Cox, M., Graves, A., & Pickering, A. (1999). Hippocampal involvement in spatial and working memory: A structural MRI analysis of patients with unilateral mesial temporal lobe sclerosis. Brain and Cognition, 41, 39–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrahams, S., Pickering, A., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1997). Spatial memory deficits in patients with unilateral damage to the right hippocampal formation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 11–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, J.E. & Sufka, K.J. (1993). Cerebral lateralization in homosexual males: A preliminary EEG investigation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 269–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, L.S. & Gorski, R.A. (1992). Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 89, 7199–7202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astur, R.S., Ortiz, M.L., & Sutherland, R.J. (1998). A characterization of performance by men and women in virtual Morris water task: A large and reliable sex difference. Behavioral Brain Research, 93, 185–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astur, R.S., Taylor, L.B., Mamelak, A.N., Philpott, L., & Sutherland, R.J. (2002). Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behavioral Brain Research, 132, 77–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, J.M. & Zucker, K.J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behaviour and sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis and quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 31, 43–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byne, W., Tobet, S., Mattiace, L.A., Lasco, M.S., Kemether, E., Edgar, M.A., Morgello, S., Buchsbaum, M.S., & Jones, L.B. (2001). The interstitial nuclei of the human anterior hypothalamus: An investigation of variation with sex, sexual orientation and HIV status. Hormones and Behavior, 40, 86–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, E. (1987). Assessment of sexual orientation. Journal of Homosexuality, 14, 9–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collaer, M.L. & Hines, M. (1995). Human behavioural sex differences: A role for gonadal hormones during early development? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 55–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eals, M. & Silverman, I. (1994). The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: Proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 95–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, E. & Ames, M.A. (1987). Neurohormonal functioning and sexual orientation: A theory of homosexuality–heterosexuality. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 233–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filipek, P.A., Richelme, C., Kennedy, D.N., & Caviness, V.S., Jr. (1994). The young adult human brain: An MRI-based morphometric analysis. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 344–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederiske, M.E., Lu, A., Aylward, E., Barta, P., & Pearlson, G. (1999). Sex differences in the inferior parietal lobule. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 896–901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giedd, J.N., Castellanos, F.X., Rajapakse, J.C., Vaituzis, A.C., & Rapoport, J.L. (1997). Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 21, 1185–1201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gladue, B.A. & Bailey, J.M. (1995). Spatial ability, handedness and human sexual orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20, 487–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gladue, B.A., Beatty, W.W., Larson, J., & Staton, R.D. (1990). Sexual orientation and spatial ability in men and women. Psychobiology, 18, 101–108.Google Scholar
Gron, G., Wunderlich, A.P., Spitzer, M., Tomczak, R., & Riepe, M.W. (2000). Brain activation during human navigation: Gender difference neural networks as substrate of performance. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 404–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gur, R.C., Alsop, D., Glahn, D., Petty, R., Swanson, R.L., Maldjian, J.A., Tureksky, B.I., Detre, J.A., Gee, J., & Gur, R.E. (2000). An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to a verbal and a spatial task. Brain and Language, 74, 157–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J.A.Y. & Kimura, D. (1995). Performance by homosexual males and females on sexually dimorphic motor tasks. Archives of Sexual Behaviour, 24, 395–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, D.F. (1992). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hamsher, K.D., Capruso, D.X., & Benton, A.L. (1992). Visuospatial judgment and right hemisphere disease. Cortex, 28, 493–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, R.D., Grut, M., Wahlin, M., Herlitz, A., Winblad, B., & Backman, L. (1995). Predicting memory performance in optimally healthy very old adults. Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 1, 57–67.Google Scholar
HMSO. (1991). Office of Population Census and Surveys. Standard occupational classification. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
Hu, S., Pattatucci, A.M.L., Patterson, C., Li, L., Fulker, D.W., Cherny, S.S., Kruglyak, L., & Hamer, D.H. (1995). Linkage between sexual orientation and chromosome Xq28 in males but not in females. Nature Genetics, 11, 248–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, T.W. & Kimura, D. (1997). Sex differences in remembering the location of objects in an array: Location shifts versus location exchanges. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 155–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnsrude, I.S., Owen, A.M., Crane, J., Milner, B., & Evans, A.C. (1999). A cognitive activation study of memory for spatial relationships. Neuropsychologia, 37, 829–841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessels, R.P.C., de Haan, E.H.F., Kappelle, L.J., & Postma, A. (2001). Varieties of human spatial memory: A meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions. Brain Research Reviews, 35, 295–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, B. & Whishaw, I.G. (1995). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. New York: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
LeVay, S. (1991). A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men. Science, 253, 1034–1037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeVay, S. (1993). The sexual brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Linn, M.C. & Petersen, A.C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lippa, R.A. (2000). Gender related traits in gay men, lesbian women, and heterosexual men and women: The virtual identity of homosexual–heterosexual diagnosticity and gender diagnosticity. Journal of Personality, 68, 899–926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E.E. & Jacklin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
McBurney, D.H., Gaulin, S.J.C., Devineni, T., & Adams, C. (1997). Superior spatial memory of women: Stronger evidence for the gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 165–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormick, C.M. & Witelson, S.F. (1991). A cognitive profile of homosexual men compared to heterosexual men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15, 459–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFadden, D. & Pasanen, E.G. (1998). Comparison of the auditory systems of heterosexuals and homosexuals: Click-evoked otoacoustic emissions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 2709–2713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffat, S.D., Hampson, E., & Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a “virtual” maze: Sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neave, N., Hamilton, C., Tildesley, N., Hutton, L., Gallagher, P., & Pickering, A. (2000). Female advantage in location memory using ecologically valid measures. Proceedings of the British Psychological Society, 8, 40.Google Scholar
Neave, N., Hamilton, C., & Gallagher, P. (1999b). Female advantage in object recall? Some methodological considerations. Proceedings of the British Psychological Society, 1, 133.Google Scholar
Neave, N., Menaged, M., & Weightman, D.R. (1999a). Sex differences in cognition: The role of testosterone and sexual orientation. Brain and Cognition, 41, 245–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunn, J.A., Graydon, F.J.X., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1999). Differential spatial memory impairment after right temporal lobectomy demonstrated using temporal titration. Brain, 122, 47–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, D.B., Archer, J., Hair, W.M., & Wu, F.C.W. (2001). Activational effects of testosterone on cognitive function in men. Neuropsychologia, 39, 1385–1394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postma, A., Izendoorn, R., & De Haan, E.H.F. (1998). Sex differences in object location memory. Brain and Cognition, 36, 334–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J.C. (1958). Guide to standard progressive matrices. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Reite, M., Sheeder, J., Richardson, D., & Teale, P. (1995). Cerebral laterality in homosexual males: Preliminary communication using magnetoencephalography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24, 585–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roof, R.L. (1993). Neonatal exogenous testosterone modifies sex difference in radial arm and Morris water maze performance in prepubescent and adult rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 53, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanders, G. & Ross-Field, L. (1987). Neuropsychological development of cognitive abilities: A new research strategy and some preliminary evidence for a sexual orientation model. International Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Save, E., Poucet, B., Foreman, N., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (1998). The contribution of the associative parietal cortex and hippocampus to spatial processing in rodents. Psychobiology, 26, 153–161.Google Scholar
Scamvougeras, A., Witelson, S.F., Branskill, M., Stanchev, P., Black, S., Cheung, G., Steiner, M., & Buck, B. (1994). Sexual orientation and anatomy of the corpus callosum. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 20, 1425.Google Scholar
Silverman, I. & Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 531–549). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, M.L. & Milner, B. (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia, 19, 781–793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, M.L. & Milner, B. (1989). Right hippocampal impairment in the recall of spatial location: Encoding deficit or rapid forgetting? Neuropsychologia, 27, 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuttle, G.E. & Pillard, R.C. (1991). Sexual orientation and cognitive abilities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20, 307–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M.P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegesin, D.J. (1998a). A neuropsychologic profile of homosexual and heterosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 27, 91–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegesin, D.J. (1998b). Event-related potentials in homosexual and heterosexual men and women: Sex dimorphic patterns in verbal asymmetries and mental rotations. Brain and Cognition, 36, 73–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, C.L., Barnett, A.M., & Meck, W.H. (1990). Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation in spatial memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 53, 1021–1024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, T.J., Pepitone, M.E., Christensen, S.E., Cooke, B.M., Huberman, A.D., Breedlove, N.J., Breedlove, T.J., Jordan, C.L., & Breedlove, S.M. (2000). Finger length ratio and sexual orientation. Nature, 404, 455–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar