Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-15T22:55:09.359Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Implicit Spoken Words and Motor Sequences Learning Are Impaired in Children with Specific Language Impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2016

Lise Desmottes*
Affiliation:
Department of Speech and Language Therapy, University of Liege, B38, Liège, Belgium
Thierry Meulemans
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Liege, B33, Liège, Belgium
Christelle Maillart
Affiliation:
Department of Speech and Language Therapy, University of Liege, B38, Liège, Belgium
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Lise Desmottes, Department of Psychology, Cognition and Behavior, University of Liege, B38, rue de l’Aunaie, 30, 4000 Liège, Belgium. E-mail: lise.desmottes@ulg.ac.be

Abstract

Objectives: This study aims to compare verbal and motor implicit sequence learning abilities in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). Methods: Forty-eight children (24 control and 24 SLI) were administered the Serial Search Task (SST), which enables the simultaneous assessment of implicit spoken words and visuomotor sequences learning. Results: Results showed that control children implicitly learned both the spoken words as well as the motor sequences. In contrast, children with SLI showed deficits in both types of learning. Moreover, correlational analyses revealed that SST performance was linked with grammatical abilities in control children but with lexical abilities in children with SLI. Conclusions: Overall, this pattern of results supports the procedural deficit hypothesis and suggests that domain general implicit sequence learning is impaired in SLI. (JINS, 2016, 22, 1–10)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chevrie-Muller, C., Maillart, C., Simon, A.M., & Fournier, S. (2010). Langage oral, langage écrit, mémoire, Attention, 2ème édition: L2MA-2. Paris: Edition du centre de psychologie appliquée (ECPA).Google Scholar
Cohen, A., Ivry, R.I., & Keele, S.W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(1), 1730.Google Scholar
Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H. (2001). Sequential learning in non-human primates. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 539546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H. (2006). Statistical learning within and between modalities pitting abstract against stimulus-specific representations. Psychological Science, 17(10), 905912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, N.A., Howard, J.H. Jr., & Howard, D.V. (2006). Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults. Experimental Brain Research, 175(1), 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L.M., Thiériault-Whalen, C.M., & Dunn, L.M. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody: Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—French edition]. Toronto, Canada: Psycan.Google Scholar
Evans, J., Saffran, J.R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 321335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabay, Y., Schiff, R., & Vakil, E. (2012). Dissociation between the procedural learning of letter names and motor sequences in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50(10), 24352441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Guillaume, M., Stefaniak, N., & Meulemans, T. (2011). Exploration of serial structure procedural learning in children with language impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabriel, A., Meulemans, T., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2014). Procedural learning across modalities in French speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(03), 747769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, A., Stefaniak, N., Maillart, C., Schmitz, X., & Meulemans, T. (2012). Procedural visual learning in children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(4), 329341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gheysen, F., Van Waelvelde, H., & Fias, W. (2011). Impaired visuo-motor sequence learning in Developmental Coordination Disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(2), 749756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goschke, T., Friederici, A.D., Kotz, S.A., & Van Kampen, A. (2001). Procedural learning in Broca’s aphasia: Dissociation between the implicit acquisition of spatio-motor and phoneme sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(3), 370388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunow, H., Spaulding, T.J., Gómez, R.L., & Plante, E. (2006). The effects of variation on learning word order rules by adults with and without language-based learning disabilities. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39, 158170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Tremblay, A., Adi-Japha, E., Veríssimo, J., Dye, C.D., & Ullman, M.T. (2011). Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 23622375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, H.J., & Bishop, D.V. (2014). Sequence‐specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17, 352365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, H.J., Tomblin, J.B., & Christiansen, M.H. (2014). Impaired statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in adolescents with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J.C., & Tomblin, J.B. (2015). Procedural learning and individual differences in language. Language, Learning, and Development, 11(3), 215236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L.B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment, (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukács, Á., & Kemény, F. (2014). Domain-general sequence learning deficit in specific language impairment. Neuropsychology, 28(3), 472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J., & Bleses, D. (2012). Declarative and procedural memory in Danish speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45(1), 4658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J.A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A.T., & Ullman, M. (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J.A., Ullman, M.T., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: Evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), 34603476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macchi, L., Descours, C., Girard, E., Guitton, E., Morel, C., Timmermans, N., &Boidein, F. (2012). Epreuve Lilloise de Discrimination Phonologique (ELDP). Lille, France: Institut d’Orthophonie Gabriel Decroix de Lille.Google Scholar
Mayor-Dubois, C., Zesiger, P., Van der Linden, M., & Roulet-Perez, E. (2012). Nondeclarative learning in children with specific language impairment: Predicting regularities in the visuomotor, phonological, and cognitive domains. Child Neuropsychology, 1, 19.Google Scholar
Nissen, M.J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ofen, N., Kao, Y.C., Sokol-Hessner, P., Kim, H., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Gabrieli, J.D. (2007). Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 11981205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plante, E., Gomez, R., & Gerken, L. (2002). Sensitivity to word order cues by normal and language/learning disabled adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 453462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J.B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X. (2007). Procedural learning and adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Language, Learning, and Development, 3(4), 269293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torkildsen, J.v.K., Dailey, N.S., Aguilar, J.M., Gómez, R., & Plante, E. (2013). Exemplar variability facilitates rapid learning of an otherwise unlearnable grammar by individuals with language-based learning disability. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 618629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M.T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M., & Pierpont, E. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M.T., & Pullman, M.Y. (2015). A compensatory role for declarative memory in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 205222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D., & Naglieri, J.A. (2006). Wechsler nonverbal scale of ability (WNV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar