Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T15:56:08.980Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Breakfast: how important is it really?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2016

Peter J Rogers*
Affiliation:
School of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of Bristol12a Priory Road, Bristol BS8 1TU, UK Email: peter.rogers@bristol.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Letters to the Editor
Copyright
Copyright © The Author 2016 

Madam

Littlecott et al.( Reference Littlecott, Moore and Moore 1 ) report a positive association between breakfast consumption and educational performance in 9- to 11-year-old children. This is an impressive study. It has a large sample size and the outcome is Summative Teacher Assessments( Reference Littlecott, Moore and Moore 2 ), which use standardised materials to evaluate student learning and skills. The study was reported widely in the print and broadcast media in the UK. These reports, like the publication itself and the accompanying press release, were mostly careful in describing the findings. None the less, a strong impression is given that learning in school benefits from eating breakfast. For example, the authors are quoted as saying that the study provides ‘robust evidence of a link between eating breakfast and doing well at school’ and that ‘embedding health improvements into the core business of the school might also deliver educational improvements’( Reference Messenger 3 ), and this is supported with quotes from a head teacher, including ‘By around 10 am they [children who miss breakfast] start flagging. It has an effect on their behaviour and concentration – some even fall asleep’( Reference Messenger 3 ). The clear implication here is that getting children to eat breakfast will improve educational performance in school. The authors also appear to suggest this in the abstract of their paper: ‘Future research should aim to explore the mechanisms by which breakfast consumption and education outcomes are linked, and understand how to promote breakfast consumption among schoolchildren( Reference Littlecott, Moore and Moore 1 ) (italics added).

While the importance of breakfast seems to be firmly embedded in our culture, it is important to recognise that this is not strongly evidenced based. An interpretation of Littlecott et al.’s( Reference Littlecott, Moore and Moore 1 ) results is that consumption of breakfast benefits educational attainment, but it is equally or more plausible that consumption of breakfast at home is a marker for the quality of a child’s home environment and parenting, and it is that which influences educational attainment. Indeed, consumption of breakfast may be a particularly good marker in this respect, better for example than free-school-meal entitlement or measures of socio-economic status, as it may reflect well at an individual level the motivation and capacity of caregivers to provide a nurturing environment for their children. (It may even predict aspiration towards educational achievement partly because the belief in the importance of breakfast for learning encourages caregivers with such aspirations to provide breakfast.) When a teacher sees a child who has missed breakfast being difficult to teach, being tired and lacking in concentration, he/she attributes this to the child not having eaten recently. That is not unreasonable, but the teacher may well be losing sight of the possibility that the child’s problems in class stem instead primarily from other aspects of the child’s home life, perhaps including lack of sufficient sleep.

So what is the evidence that eating breakfast is important for performance in school? In an earlier paper( Reference Moore, Murphy and Chaplin 4 ) from the same group on the same cohort of children (n 4500) it was reported that universal breakfast provision did not improve cognitive function v. a wait-list control group. Furthermore, no interaction was found between the effect of the intervention and socio-economic deprivation measured by free-school-meal entitlement. Only results for episodic memory were reported (briefly), but the implication is that other cognitive test outcomes showed a similar lack of effect of universal breakfast provision. Relevant intervention studies have been carried out since at least the 1980s. These present a mixed picture, particularly of the acute effects of missing breakfast in relatively well-nourished children( Reference Hoyland, Dye and Lawton 5 ). In perhaps the earliest such study( Reference Pollitt, Leibel and Greenfield 6 ), 9- to 11-year-old children who missed breakfast were tested mid-morning 18h after their last meal. Performance was little affected compared with when they ate breakfast. On a component of one task, performance was improved after missing breakfast, while on another task missing breakfast led to impaired performance in subgroup of the participants. In contrast, studies of school breakfast programmes in nutritionally at-risk populations have found more consistently positive effects of breakfast provision on educational and cognitive test outcomes, it is suggested in part due to increased school attendance that providing breakfast encourages( Reference Hoyland, Dye and Lawton 5 ).

This discrepancy between evidence and belief in the importance of breakfast for educational performance is very similar to Brown et al.’s( Reference Brown, Bohan Brown and Allison 7 ) conclusion for the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity. As they demonstrate very nicely, an association between missing breakfast and higher risk of obesity is well established, while the evidence from the relatively few relevant intervention studies does not clearly confirm or refute a causal effect. Brown et al. also show that there is considerable bias in the reporting and interpretation of this evidence in both scientific and popular publications, resulting in ‘belief beyond the evidence’ for this particular effect of breakfast. Plausibly, the correlation between missing breakfast and higher weight is explained largely by overweight and obese people trying, in their efforts to eat less, to delay eating for as long as possible in the day. In non-obese individuals, at least, this appears to succeed, as missing breakfast has been found to lead to reduced energy intake over the whole day( Reference Levitsky and Pacanowski 8 ). Such results then beg the question as to why eating breakfast is advocated for healthy weight management( 9 ). Indeed, a case could be made that if there is little or no appetite for breakfast this meal can be safely missed by those who are well nourished, including children, because this will not adversely affect performance (body energy reserves can be readily mobilised to fuel muscle and brain activity), but will likely help reduce excessive energy intake.

From the perspective of public health and informing investment in cost-effective interventions, it is important that evidence from research is examined dispassionately and in the round. This includes evidence concerning the importance or otherwise of eating breakfast.

Acknowledgements

Financial support: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: P.J.R. is the exclusive author of the manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: Not applicable.

References

1. Littlecott, HJ, Moore, GF, Moore, L et al. (2015) Association between breakfast consumption and educational outcomes in 9–11-year-old children. Public Health Nutr (Epublication ahead of print version).Google Scholar
2. Littlecott, HJ, Moore, GF, Moore, L et al. (2015) Association between breakfast consumption and educational outcomes in 9–11-year-old children – CORRIGENDUM. Public Health Nutr (Epublication ahead of print version).Google Scholar
3. Messenger, S (2015) Healthy breakfast ‘help pupils do well’. http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-wales-34834832 (accessed November 2015).Google Scholar
4. Moore, GF, Murphy, S, Chaplin, K et al. (2014) Impacts of the Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative on socio-economic inequalities in breakfast consumption among 9–11-year-old schoolchildren in Wales. Public Health Nutr 17, 12801289.Google Scholar
5. Hoyland, A, Dye, L & Lawton, CL (2009) A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutr Res Rev 22, 220243.Google Scholar
6. Pollitt, E, Leibel, RL & Greenfield, D (1981) Brief fasting, stress, and cognition in children. Am J Clin Nutr 34, 15261533.Google Scholar
7. Brown, AW, Bohan Brown, MM & Allison, DB (2013) Belief beyond the evidence: using the proposed effect of breakfast to show 2 practises that distort scientific evidence. Am J Clin Nutr 98, 12981308.Google Scholar
8. Levitsky, DA & Pacanowski, CR (2011) Effect of skipping breakfast on subsequent energy intake. Physiol Behav 119, 916.Google Scholar
9. NHS Choices (2015) Healthy breakfasts (for people who hate breakfast). http://www.nhs.uk/Livewell/loseweight/Pages/Healthybreakfasts.aspx (accessed October 2015).Google Scholar