Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T15:03:34.278Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Binge Drinkers Are Fast, Able to Stop – but They Fail to Adjust

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2015

Ragnhild Bø*
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Martin Aker
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Joël Billieux
Affiliation:
Laboratory for Experimental Psychopathology, Psychological Science Research Institute, Catholic University of Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
Nils Inge Landrø
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Ragnhild Bø, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 1094 Blindern, 0317, Oslo, Norway. E-mail: ragnhild.bo@psykologi.uio.no

Abstract

Binge drinking leads to brain damage. However, at present few studies have taken into account the continuity in the binge drinking phenomenon, and treated binge drinking as a clearly separable category from other types of drinking patterns. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether severity of binge drinking can predict specific neurocognitive changes in healthy young adults. A total of 121 students aged 18 to 25 were assessed by means of the three last questions of the Alcohol Use Questionnaire combined into binge score. The binge score was entered as a predictor of cognitive performance of the CANTAB Stop Signal Task including reaction time, inhibition processing time, and response adjustment. Anxiety and depression symptoms were also measured. Binge score significantly predicted less adjustment following failures, and faster reaction times. Binge score did not predict inhibition performance. Symptoms of depression and anxiety were not significantly related to binge score. Binge drinking in healthy young adults predicts impairment in response adjustment and fast reaction time, but is unrelated to inhibition. The study supports the view that binge drinking is a continuous phenomenon, rather than discrete category, and the findings are possibly shedding light on why binge drinkers continue their drinking pattern despite negative consequences. (JINS, 2016, 22, 38–46)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Babor, T.F., Higgins-Biddle, J.C., Saunders, J.B., & Monteiro, M.G. (2001). AUDIT - Alcohol use identification test. Guidlines for use in primary care (2nd ed). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Beck, A.T., Brown, G., Epstein, N., & Steer, R.A. (1988). An inventory for measuring calinical anxiety - Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.56.6.893 Google Scholar
Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W.F. (1996). Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67(3), 588597. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonomo, Y.A., Bowes, G., Coffey, C., Carlin, J.B., & Patton, G.C. (2004). Teenage drinking and the onset of alcohol dependence: a cohort study over seven years. Addiction, 99(12), 15201528. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00846.x Google Scholar
Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S., & Cohen, J.D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, K.E., & Polich, J. (2009). Binge drinking in young adults: Data, definitions, and determinants. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 142156. doi:10.1037/a0014414 Google Scholar
Courtney, K.E., & Polich, J. (2010). Binge drinking effects on EEG in young adult humans. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 7(5), 23252336. doi:10.3390/ijerph7052325 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cranford, J.A., McCabe, S.E., & Boyd, C.J. (2006). A new measure of binge drinking: Prevalence and correlates in a probability sample of undergraduates. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 30(11), 18961905. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00234.x Google Scholar
Crego, A., Rodriguez-Holguin, S., Parada, M., Mota, N., Corral, M., & Cadaveira, F. (2010). Reduced anterior prefrontal cortex activation in young binge drinkers during a visual working memory task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 109(1-3), 4556. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.020 Google Scholar
Crego, A., Rodriguez Holguin, S., Parada, M., Mota, N., Corral, M., & Cadaveira, F. (2009). Binge drinking affects attentional and visual working memory processing in young university students. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(11), 18701879. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01025.x Google Scholar
Czapla, M., Simon, J.J., Friederich, H.C., Herpertz, S.C., Zimmermann, P., & Loeber, S. (2015). Is binge drinking in young adults associated with an alcohol-specific impairment of response inhibition? European Addiction Research, 21(2), 105113.Google Scholar
Evans, J.S. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 454459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goudriaan, A.E., Grekin, E.R., & Sher, K.J. (2011). Decision making and response inhibition as predictors of heavy alcohol use: A prospective study. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(6), 10501057. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01437.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerri, C., & Pascual, M. (2010). Mechanisms involved in the neurotoxic, cognitive, and neurobehavioral effects of alcohol consumption during adolescence. Alcohol, 44(1), 1526. doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.10.003 Google Scholar
Hallgren, M.S., & McCrady, B.S. (2013). Interference in the alcohol Stroop task with college student binge drinkers. Journal of Behavioral Health, 2(2), 112119. doi:10.5455/jbh.20130224082728 Google Scholar
Hartley, D.E., Elsabagh, S., & File, S.E. (2004). Binge drinking and sex: Effects on mood and cognitive function in healthy young volunteers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 18(3), A41A41.Google Scholar
Henges, A.L., & Marczinski, C.A. (2012). Impulsivity and alcohol consumption in young social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 37(2), 217220. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.09.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsson, P. (2012). The relationship between affective associations with alcohol and binge drinking. Journal of Substance Use, 17(1), 4150. doi:10.3109/14659891.2010.519419 Google Scholar
Lannoy, S., Billieux, J., & Maurage, P. (2014). Beyond inhibition: A dual-process perspective to renew the exploration of binge drinking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 405. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00405 Google Scholar
Lawrence, A.J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N.A., Sahakian, B.J., & Clark, L. (2009). Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology, 207(1), 163172. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1645-x Google Scholar
Li, C.-S. R., Milivojevic, V., Kemp, K., Hong, K., & Sinha, R. (2006). Performance monitoring and stop signal inhibition in abstinent patients with cocaine dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 85(3), 205212. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.04.008 Google Scholar
Loeber, S., & Duka, T. (2009). Acute alcohol decreases performance of an instrumental response to avoid aversive consequences in social drinkers. Psychopharmacology, 205(4), 577587. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1565-9 Google Scholar
Logan, G.D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A user guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in memory and language (pp. 189239). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lopez-Caneda, E., Cadaveira, F., Crego, A., Gomez-Suarez, A., Corral, M., Parada, M., & Holguin, S.R. (2012). Hyperactivation of right inferior frontal cortex in young binge drinkers during response inhibition: A follow-up study. Addiction, 107(10), 17961808. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03908.x Google Scholar
Mashhoon, Y., Czerkawski, C., Crowley, D.J., Cohen-Gilbert, J.E., Sneider, J.T., & Silveri, M.M. (2014). Binge alcohol consumption in emerging adults: Anterior cingulate cortical “thinness” is associated with alcohol use patterns. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 38(7), 19551964. doi:10.1111/acer.12475 Google Scholar
Maurage, P., Joassin, F., Speth, A., Modave, J., Philippot, P., & Campanella, S. (2012). Cerebral effects of binge drinking: Respective influences of global alcohol intake and consumption pattern. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(5), 892901. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.018 Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J.A. (1978). Questionnaire measure of habitual alcohol use. Psychological Reports, 43(3), 803806.Google Scholar
Montgomery, C., Fisk, J.E., Murphy, P.N., Ryland, I., & Hilton, J. (2012). The effects of heavy social drinking on executive function: A systematic review and meta-analytic study of existing literature and new empirical findings. Human Psychopharmacology, 27(2), 187199. doi:10.1002/hup.1268 Google Scholar
Nederkoorn, C., Baltus, M., Guerrieri, R., & Wiers, R.W. (2009). Heavy drinking is associated with deficient response inhibition in women but not in men. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93(3), 331336. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.015 Google Scholar
Ng, J., Chan, H.Y., & Schlaghecken, F. (2012). Dissociating effects of subclinical anxiety and depression on cognitive control. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8(1), 3849. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0100-6 Google Scholar
Nigg, J.T., Glass, J.M., Wong, M.M., Poon, E., Jester, J.M., Fitzgerald, H.E., & Zucker, R.A. (2004). Neuropsychological executive functioning in children at elevated risk for alcoholism: Findings in early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(2), 302314. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.113.2.302 Google Scholar
Plant, M.A., Plant, M.L., Miller, P., Gmel, G., & Kuntsche, S. (2009). The social consequences of binge drinking: A comparison of young adults in six European countries. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 28(4), 294308. doi:10.1080/10550880903182978 Google Scholar
Rieger, M., & Gauggel, S. (1999). Inhibitory after-effects in the stop signal paradigm. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 509518. doi:10.1348/000712699161585 Google Scholar
Rose, A.K., & Grunsell, L. (2008). The subjective, rather than the disinhibiting, effects of alcohol are related to binge drinking. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(6), 10961104. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00672.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Roige, S., Baro, V., Trick, L., Pena-Oliver, Y., Stephens, D.N., & Duka, T. (2014). Exaggerated waiting impulsivity associated with human binge drinking, and high alcohol consumption in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(13), 29192927. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.151 Google Scholar
Saunders, J.B., Aasland, O.G., Babor, T.F., de la Fuente, J.R., & Grant, I. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorder identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption – II. Addiction, 88, 791804.Google Scholar
Scaife, J.C., & Duka, T. (2009). Behavioural measures of frontal lobe function in a population of young social drinkers with binge drinking pattern. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93(3), 354362. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2009.05.015 Google Scholar
Smith, G.T., & Anderson, K.G. (2001). Personality and learning factors combine to create risk for adolescent problem drinking: A model and suggestions for interventions. In P.M. Monti, S.M. Colby & T.A. O’Leary (Eds.), Adolescents, alcohol and substance abuse: Reaching teens through brief interventions (pp. 109144). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stephens, D.N., Ripley, T.L., Borlikova, G., Schubert, M., Albrecht, D., Hogarth, L., & Duka, T. (2005). Repeated ethanol exposure and withdrawal impairs human fear conditioning and depresses long-term potentiation in rat amygdala and hippocampus. Biological Psychiatry, 58(5), 392400. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.025 Google Scholar
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220247. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thakkar, K.N., Congdon, E., Poldrack, R.A., Sabb, F.W., London, E.D., Cannon, T.D., & Bilder, R.M. (2014). Women are more sensitive than men to prior trial events on the Stop-signal task. British Journal of Psychology, 105(2), 254272. doi:10.1111/bjop.12034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townshend, J.M., & Duka, T. (2002). Patterns of alcohol drinking in a population of young social drinkers: A comparison of questionnaire and diary measures. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 37(2), 187192. doi:10.1093/alcalc/37.2.187 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townshend, J.M., & Duka, T. (2005). Binge drinking, cognitive performance and mood in a population of young social drinkers. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(3), 317325. doi:10.1097/01.alc.0000156453.05028.f5 Google Scholar
Townshend, J.M., Kambouropoulos, N., Griffin, A., Hunt, F.J., & Milani, R.M. (2014). Binge drinking, reflection impulsivity, and unplanned sexual behavior: Impaired decision-making in young social drinkers. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 38(4), 11431150. doi:10.1111/acer.12333 Google Scholar
Tsai, J., Ford, E.S., Li, C.Y., Pearson, W.S., & Zhao, G.X. (2010). Binge drinking and suboptimal self-rated health among adult drinkers. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 34(8), 14651471. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01231.x Google Scholar
Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G.D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: Associative learning in the Go/No-Go and Stop-Signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 137(4), 649672. doi:10.1037/a0013170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbruggen, F., Logan, G.D., Liefooghe, B., & Vandierendonck, A. (2008). Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: Repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 413426. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.413 Google Scholar
Weissenborn, R., & Duka, T. (2003). Acute alcohol effects on cognitive function in social drinkers: Their relationship to drinking habits. Psychopharmacology, 165(3), 306312. doi:10.1007/s00213-002-1281-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed