Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T16:42:21.878Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Voluntary and involuntary processes affect the production of verbal and non-verbal signals by the human voice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2014

Carolyn McGettigan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham TW20 0EX, United Kingdom. Carolyn.McGettigan@rhul.ac.ukwww.carolynmcgettigan.com
Sophie Kerttu Scott
Affiliation:
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom. sophie.scott@ucl.ac.ukhttps://sites.google.com/site/speechskscott/

Abstract

We argue that a comprehensive model of human vocal behaviour must address both voluntary and involuntary aspects of articulate speech and non-verbal vocalizations. Within this, plasticity of vocal output should be acknowledged and explained as part of the mature speech production system.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Blank, S. C., Bird, H., Turkheimer, F. & Wise, R. J. (2003) Speech production after stroke: The role of the right pars opercularis. Annals of Neurology 54(3):310–20. doi: 10.1002/ana.10656.Google Scholar
Blank, S. C., Scott, S. K., Murphy, K., Warburton, E. & Wise, R. J. (2002) Speech production: Wernicke, Broca and beyond. Brain 125 (Pt. 8): 1829–38.Google Scholar
Dronkers, N. F. (1996) A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 384(6605):159–61. doi: 10.1038/384159a0.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., MacKay, I. R. & Meador, D. (1999a) Native Italian speakers' perception and production of English vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106(5):2973–87.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H. & Liu, S. (1999b) Age constraints on Second-Language Acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language 41:78104.Google Scholar
Harrington, J., Palethorpe, S. & Watson, C. I. (2000) Does the Queen speak the Queen's English? Elizabeth II's traditional pronunciation has been influenced by modern trends. Nature 408(6815):927–28. doi: 10.1038/35050160.Google Scholar
Hawkins, S. & Smith, R. (2001) Polysp: A polysystemic, phonetically-rich approach to speech understanding. Italian Journal of Linguistics 13:99188.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2003) Blood, sweat, and fears: The autonomic architecture of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1000:348–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGettigan, C., Eisner, F., Agnew, Z. K., Manly, T., Wisbey, D. & Scott, S. K. (2013) T'ain't what you say, it's the way that you say it – Left insula and inferior frontal cortex work in interaction with superior temporal regions to control the performance of vocal impersonations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25(11):1875–86. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00427.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. & Emmorey, K. (2006) Laughter among deaf signers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11(4):403409. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enl008.Google Scholar
Ruch, W. & Ekman, P. (2001) The expressive pattern of laughter. In: Emotion, qualia, and consciousness, ed. Kaszniak, A. W., pp. 426–43. World Scientific.Google Scholar
Van Lancker, D. & Cummings, J. L. (1999) Expletives: Neurolinguistic and neurobehavioral perspectives on swearing. Brain Research Reviews 31:83104.Google Scholar
Wattendorf, E., Westermann, B., Fiedler, K., Kaza, E., Lotze, M. & Celio, M. R. (2013) Exploration of the neural correlates of ticklish laughter by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex 23(6):1280–89. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs094.Google Scholar