Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T19:24:24.129Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Acoustic-phonetic differences between infant- and adult-directed speech: the role of stress and utterance position

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2014

YUANYUAN WANG
Affiliation:
Linguistics, Purdue University
AMANDA SEIDL*
Affiliation:
Speech Language & Hearing Sciences, Purdue University
ALEJANDRINA CRISTIA
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, CNRS, IEC-ENS, EHESS
*
[*]Address for correspondence: Amanda Seidl, Speech Language & Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, 500 Oval Dr., West Lafayette, IN 47907. e-mail: aseidl@purdue.edu

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that infant-directed speech (IDS) differs from adult-directed speech (ADS) on a variety of dimensions. The aim of the current study was to investigate whether acoustic differences between IDS and ADS in English are modulated by prosodic structure. We compared vowels across the two registers (IDS, ADS) in both stressed and unstressed syllables, and in both utterance-medial and -final positions. Vowels in target bisyllabic trochees in the speech of twenty mothers of 4- and 11-month-olds were analyzed. While stressed and unstressed vowels differed between IDS and ADS for a measure of F0, and trended in similar directions for vowel peripherality, neither set differed in duration. These profiles held for both utterance-medial and -final words.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Albin, D. D. & Echols, C. H. (1996). Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech. Infant Behavior and Development 19, 401–18.Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N., Woodward, J., LaMendola, N. & Bever, T. G. (1996). Models of word segmentation in fluent maternal speech to infants. In Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K. (eds), Signal to syntax: boostrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, 117–34. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). languageR: data sets and functions with ‘Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics’. R package version 0.953.Google Scholar
Bates, D. & Sarkar, D. (2007). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes.Google Scholar
Bernstein, R. N. (1986). Durational cues which mark clauses boundaries in mother–child speech. Journal of Phonetics 12(2), 303–9.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2011). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.2.28) [Computer program], online: <http://www.praat.org/> (last accessed 11 July 2011).+(last+accessed+11+July+2011).>Google Scholar
Bortfeld, H. & Morgan, J. L. (2010). Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition. Cognitive Psychology 60(4), 241–66.Google Scholar
Bradlow, A. R., Torretta, G. M. & Pisoni, D. B. (1996). Intelligibility of normal speech I: global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech Communication 20(3/4), 255–72.Google Scholar
Church, R. (2002). Prosodic modification in infant-directed speech. Unpublished master's thesis, University of British Columbia, Canada.Google Scholar
Cristia, A. (2013). Input to language: the phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass 7(3), 157–70.Google Scholar
Cristia, A. (2014). Report: Acoustic cues to allophony. Online: <https://sites.google.com/site/acrsta/Home/nsf_allophones_corpora>..>Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Mintz, T. H. & Christiansen, M. H. (2005). Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation. Cognition 96, 233–62.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. & Norris, D. G. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14, 113–21.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Boersma, P., Rauber, A. S. & Bion, R. A. H. (2009). A cross-dialect acoustic description of vowels: Brazilian and European Portuguese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 126, 1379–93.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 8, 181–95.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27, 209–21.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20, 104–13.Google Scholar
Fry, D. B. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech 1, 126–52.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Gleitman, H., Landau, B. & Wanner, E. (1988). Where learning begins: initial representations for language learning. In Newmeyer, F. J. (ed.), Linguistics: the Cambridge survey: Vol. 3. Language: psychological and biological aspects (pp. 150193). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Houston, D., Santelmann, L. & Jusczyk, P. (2004). English-learning infants’ segmentation of trisyllabic words from fluent speech. Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 97136.Google Scholar
Jacobson, S., Ward, N. & Sundare, M. (2011). Acoustic correlates of stress in Spanish infant-directed speech. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 130(4), 2428.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M. & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology 39(3/4), 159207.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. & Thompson, E. (1978). Perception of a phonetic contrast in multisyllabic utterances by 2-month-old infants. Perception and Psychophysics 23(2), 105–9.Google Scholar
Kaplan, P. S., Bachorowski, J. A., Smoski, M. J. & Hudenko, W. J. (2002). Infants of depressed mothers, although competent learners, fail to learn in response to their own mothers’ infant-directed speech. Psychological Sciences 13(3), 268–71.Google Scholar
Kitamura, C. & Burnham, D. (2003), Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy 4(1), 85110.Google Scholar
Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D. & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2002). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development 24(4), 372–92.Google Scholar
Kondaurova, M. V. & Bergeson, T. R. (2011). The effects of age and infant hearing status on maternal use of prosodic cues for clause boundaries in speech. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research (Online) 54(3), 740754A.Google Scholar
Kondaurova, M. V., Bergeson, T. R. & Xu, H. (2013). Age-related changes in prosodic features of maternal speech to prelingually deaf infants with cochlear implants. Infancy, 18(5). Online: <doi: 10.1111/infa.12010>.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lipsey, M. W. & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Liu, H., Tsao, F. M. & Kuhl, P. K. (2009). Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language 36(4), 909–22.Google Scholar
McMurray, B., Kovack-Lesh, K. A., Goodwin, D. & McEchron, W. (2013). Infant directed speech and the development of speech perception: enhancing development or an unintended consequence? Cognition 129(2), 362–78.Google Scholar
Papousek, H. & Papousek, M. (1979). Early ontogeny of human social interaction: its biological roots and social dimensions. In Von Granach, M., Foppa, K., Lepenies, W. & Ploog, D. (eds), Human ethology: claims and limits of a new discipline (pp. 456478). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Papousek, M., Papousek, H. & Symmes, D. (1991). The meanings and melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behavior and Development 14, 415–40.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Seidl, A. (2007). Infants’ use and weighting of prosodic cues in clause segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language 57, 2448.Google Scholar
Shute, B. & Wheldall, K. (1989). Pitch alterations in British motherese: some preliminary acoustic data. Journal of Child Language 16(03), 503–12.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition (pp. 3149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., MacKain, K. & Spieker, S. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology 18, 727–35.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Speiker, S., Barnett, R. K. & Mackain, K. (1983). The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language 10, 115.Google Scholar
Swanson, L. A. (1990). Vowel duration in mothers’ speech to young children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Retrieved from U.M.I Database (AAT 9104712).Google Scholar
Thanavisuth, C. & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (1998). Acoustic quality of Thai infant directed speech and adult directed speech. In Mannell, Robert & Robert-Ribes, J. (eds), Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing-ICSLP 98 , 445–8.Google Scholar
Thiessen, E. D. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy 7(1), 5371.Google Scholar
Turk, A. E. & White, L. (1999). Structural influences on accentual lengthening in English. Journal of Phonetics 27(2), 171206.Google Scholar
Wightman, C. W., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Ostendorf, M. & Prince, P. J. (1992). Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic boundaries. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 91(3), 1707–17.Google Scholar