Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-26T20:09:45.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Towards a dialect-neutral assessment instrument for the language skills of Afrikaans-speaking children: the role of socioeconomic status*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 February 2012

FRENETTE SOUTHWOOD*
Affiliation:
Stellenbosch University
*
Address for correspondence: Dept of General Linguistics, Stellenbosch University, Private Bag x1, Matieland, 7602, South Africa. tel: +27 21 8082010; fax: +27 21 8082009. e-mail: fs@sun.ac.za

Abstract

The aims of the study were to establish whether there is a correlation between the socioeconomic background of Afrikaans-speaking children and their performance on a dialect-neutral language test, and to ascertain whether the allowance the test currently makes for parental education level is sufficient. The Afrikaans version of the Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation (Seymour, Roeper & de Villiers, 2005a) was administered to 231 Afrikaans-speaking children age 4 ; 0 to 9 ; 11 from various socioeconomic backgrounds. A positive correlation was found between the composite language scores as well as the scores for each of the language domains (syntax, pragmatics, semantics) and the primary female caregivers' highest level of education. Children with father figures present did not outperform those without. It appears that the original manner of accommodating parental education level in interpreting the children's language scores on the test is sufficient and need not be refined for the South African context.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This material is based on work financially supported by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation and the National Research Foundation of South Africa. Any opinion, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author and therefore the Foundations do not accept any liability in regard thereto. The author would like to thank Dr Katharina Bremer (Ruprecht Karls University Heidelberg) for her valuable input.

References

REFERENCES

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) (1983). Position paper: Social dialects and implications of the position on social dialects. ASHA 25(9).Google Scholar
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) (2007–2010). Hearing screening. Retrieved from www.asha.org/public/hearing/testing/.Google Scholar
Black, M. M., Dubowitz, H. & Starr, R. H. Jr. (1999). African American fathers in low income, urban families: Development, behavior, and home environment. Child Development 70, 967–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., Faragher, B., Simkin, Z., Knox, E. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Predicting pathways of SLI: What differentiates the best and the worst outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 42, 1013–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G., Klebanov, P. K. & Sealand, N. (1993). Do neighborhoods influence child and adolescent development? American Journal of Sociology 99, 353–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buitendag, M. M. (1994). Afrikaanse reseptiewe woordeskattoets [Afrikaans receptive vocabulary test]. Pretoria: Human Sciences Research Council.Google Scholar
Christenson, S., Rounds, T. & Gorney, D. (1992). Family factors and student achievement: An avenue to increase students' success. School Psychology Quarterly 7, 178206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, H. K. & Washington, J. A. (2000). An assessment battery for identifying language impairments in African American children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 43, 366–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, A. E. & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology 33, 934–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S. (1976). Language development. Structure and function, 2nd edn.New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
de Villiers, P. (2004). Assessing pragmatic skills in elicited production. Seminars in Speech and Language 25, 5771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantuzzo, J., Davis, G. & Ginsburg, M. (1995). Effects of parental involvement in isolation or in combination with peer tutoring on student self-concept and mathematics achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology 87, 272–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flores, R. L. (2004). The effect of poverty on young children's ability to organize everyday events. Journal of Children & Poverty 10, 99118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1982). Questioning at home and at school: A comparative study. In Spindler, G. (ed.), Doing the ethnography of schooling: Educational anthropology in action, 234–36. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 74, 1368–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, R. J. (2000). An introduction to Classification and Regression Tree (CART) Analysis. Paper presented at the 2000 Annual Meeting of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine, San Francisco, California.Google Scholar
Locke, A., Ginsborg, J. & Peers, I. (2002). Development and disadvantage: Implications for the early years and beyond. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 37, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manzo, A. V., Manzo, U. C. & Thomas, M. M. (2006). Rationale for systematic vocabulary development: Antidote for state mandates. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy 40, 610–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormick, K. (2002). Language in Cape Town's District Six. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Neuman, S. B. & Celano, D. (2001). Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities: An ecological study of four neighborhoods. Reading Research Quarterly 36, 826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D. & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science 10, 464–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oetting, J. B. (2005). Assessing language in children who speak a nonmainstream dialect of English. In Ball, M. J. (ed.), Clinical sociolinguistics, 180–92. Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pretorius, A. (1989). Die Afrikaanse semantiese taalevalueringsmedium [The Afrikaans semantic language evaluation medium]. Pretoria: Author.Google Scholar
Raizada, R. D. S., Richards, T. L., Meltzoff, A. & Kuhl, P. K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage 40, 1392–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, A. (1992). Comparing measures of parental involvement and their effects on academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 7, 441–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romaine, S. (1984). The language of children and adolescents. The acquisition of communicative competence. Oxford/New York: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D. & Gannon-Rowley, T. (2002). Assessing ‘neighborhood effects’: Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology 28, 443–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T. & de Villiers, J. G. (2004). Conclusion, future directions, implications and remediation. Seminars in Speech and Language 25, 113–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T. W. & de Villiers, J. (with de Villiers, P.) (2005a). Diagnostic evaluation of language variation. Norm referenced. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp, Pearson.Google Scholar
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T. W. & de Villiers, J. (2005b). Diagnostic evaluation of language variation. Examiner's manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc.Google Scholar
Seymour, H. N. & Seymour, C. M. (1977). A therapeutic model for communicative disorders among Black English speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 42, 247–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shinn, M. (1978). Father absence and children's cognitive development. Psychological Bulletin 85, 295324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinatra, R. (2008). Creating a culture of vocabulary acquisition for children living in poverty. Journal of Children and Poverty 14, 173–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S. & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Southwood, F. & Van Dulm, O. (2009). Die poeliesman het 'n gun: Die prestasie van Kaapssprekende plattelandse leerders op 'n Afrikaans-medium taaltoets wat dialek-neutraal sou wees [The policeman has a gun: The performance of ‘Kaaps’-speaking rural learners in a purportedly dialect-neutral Afrikaans-medium language]. LitNet Akademies 6, (2), available at: http://www.oulitnet.co.za/newlitnet/pdf/la/LA_6_3.pdf.Google Scholar
Statistics South Africa (2003). Census 2001. Census in brief. (No. 03-02-02-2001). Pretoria: Statistics South Africa.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Hardy, J. C. & Hein, H. A. (1991). Predicting poor communication status in preschool children using risk factors present at birth. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 34, 10961105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families (January 2010). Head Start impact study. Final report. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Van Dulm, O. & Southwood, F. (2008). Toward a dialect-neutral Afrikaans-medium child language assessment instrument: Test item development. Language Matters 39, 300315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Rooyen, D. & Jordaan, H. (2009). An aspect of language for academic purposes in secondary education: Complex sentence comprehension by learners in an integrated Gauteng school. South African Journal of Education 29, 271–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vernon-Feagans, L. (1996). Children's talk in communities and classrooms. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Vernon-Feagans, L., Scheffner Hammer, C., Miccio, A. & Manlove, E. (2002). Early language and literacy skills in low-income African-American and Hispanic children. In Neuman, S. & Dickinson, D. (eds), Handbook of early literacy research, 192210. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Vorster, J. (1980). Toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie [Test for oral language production]. Pretoria: South African Institute for Psychological and Psychometric Research.Google Scholar
Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B. & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development 65, 606621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westby, C. (1994). Communication refinement in school age and adolescence. In Haynes, O. & Shulman, B. (eds), Communication development: Foundations, processes & clinical applications, 311–60. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J. (1997). Language processes in context: Language learning in children reared in poverty. In Adamson, L. B. & Romski, M. A. (eds), Research on communication and language disorders: Contribution to theories of language development, 233–66. Balitmore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Wright, C., Diener, M. & Kay, S. C. (2000). School readiness of low-income children at risk for school failure. Journal of Children & Poverty 6, 99117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar