Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T20:44:11.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The development of young Chinese children's morphological awareness: The role of semantic relatedness and morpheme type

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2011

MEILING HAO
Affiliation:
Beijing Normal University and Beijing Language and Culture University
XI CHEN*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
VEDRAN DRONJIC
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
HUA SHU
Affiliation:
Beijing Normal University
RICHARD C. ANDERSON
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Xi Chen, Ontario institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto, 252 Bloor Street West, 9-221, Toronto, ON M5S 1V6, Canada. E-mail: xchenbumgardner@gmail.com

Abstract

The research reported in this paper investigated the effects of semantic relatedness of words (closely related vs. distantly related) and morpheme type (free morpheme vs. bound morpheme) on young Chinese children's homophone awareness, an aspect of morphological awareness, in two experiments. The first experiment was a cross-sectional study including 39 children in a beginning kindergarten class, 39 children in an intermediate kindergarten class, and 36 children in a senior kindergarten class. The second experiment was a 7-month longitudinal study involving 43 first graders and 50 second graders at the beginning of the study. In both experiments, the children judged whether orally presented words shared the same morpheme or contained homophonous morphemes. The results suggest that homophone awareness emerges in Chinese children in the kindergarten years. Children's morpheme identification is facilitated by the semantic proximity of words that share a morpheme, and awareness of free morphemes is developed before that of bound morphemes. Furthermore, although semantic relatedness is the most prominent factor in kindergarten, its effect varies as a function of morpheme type in the early primary grades. Our research sheds light on the developmental course of morphological awareness and the factors that influence it.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aitchison, J. (2003). Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10, Serial No. 238), 1165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauvillain, C. (1996). The integration of morphological and whole-word form information during eye fixations on prefixed and suffixed words. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 801820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2007). Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 57, 4577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Berko, J. (1960). Word association and the acquisition of a grammar. Child Development, 31, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 239253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casalis, S., & Louis-Alexandre, M. F. (2000). Morphological analysis, phonological analysis and learning to read French: a longitudinal study. Read and Writing, 12, 303335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cazden, C. R. (1974). Play with language and metalinguistic awareness: One dimension of language experience. Urban Review, 7, 2839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceccagno, A., & Basciano, B. (2007). Compound headedness in Chinese: An analysis of neologisms. Morphology, 17, 207231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Hao, M. L., Geva, E., Zhu, J., & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in Chinese children's vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing, 22, 615631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Y., & Zheng, Y. (2000). 吕 叔 湘 著 《 汉 语 语 法 分 析 问 题 》 助 读 [Companion to Lu Shuxiang's analysis of Chinese grammar]. Beijing: Chinese Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Chung, W.-L., & Hu, C.-F. (2007). Morphological awareness and learning to read Chinese. Reading and Writing, 20, 441461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2001). Emergent categories in first language acquisition. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Conceptual development and language acquisition (pp. 379405). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dronjic, V. (2011). Mandarin Chinese compounds, their representation, and processing in the visual modality. Writing Systems Research, 3, 521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabb, N. (2005). Compounding. In Spencer, A. & Zwicky, A. M. (Eds.), The handbook of morphology (3rd ed., pp. 6683). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B. (2000). Are morphological effects distinguishable from the effects of shared meaning and shared form? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 14311444.Google ScholarPubMed
Feldman, L. B., & Soltano, E. G. (1999). Morphological priming: The role of prime duration, semantic transparency, and affix position. Brain and Language, 68, 3339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, L. B., Soltano, E. G., Pastizzo, M. J., & Francis, S. E. (2004). What do graded effects of semantic transparency reveal about morphological processing? Brain and Language, 90, 1730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, K. I., & Azuma, T. (2000). Masked priming for prefixed words with bound stems: Does submit prime permit? Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 539561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A. E., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157188). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2004). Effect of relation availability on the interpretation and access of familiar noun–noun compounds. Brain and Language, 90, 478486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hao, M., Shu, H., & Xing, A. (2011). Spoken vocabulary corpus for Chinese children aged 3 to 6 years old. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Hao, M., Shu, H., Xing, A., & Li, P. (2008). Early vocabulary inventory for Mandarin Chinese. Behaviour Research Methods, 40, 728733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Institute of Language Teaching and Research. (1986). 现 代 汉 语 频 率 词 典 [Modern Chinese frequency dictionary]. Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Press.Google Scholar
Ku, Y. M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L.-J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roeloefs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, W., Anderson, R. C., Nagy, W., & Zhang, H. (2002). Facets of metalinguistic awareness that contribute to Chinese literacy. In Li, W., Gaffney, J. S., & Packard, J. L. (Eds.), Chinese children's reading acquisition: Theoretical and pedagogical issues (pp. 87106). London: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Libben, G. (1998). Semantic transparency in the processing of compounds: Consequences for representation, processing, and impairment. Brain and Language, 61, 3044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, P. D., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). What is morphological awareness? Tapping lexical compounding awareness in Chinese third graders.Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 6273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Tyler, L. K., Waksler, R., & Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101, 333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., Chow, B. W.-Y., Chow, C. S.-L., & Choi, L. (2006). Metalinguistic skills and vocabulary knowledge in Chinese (L1) and English (L2). Reading and Writing, 19, 695716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children's Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Muse, A., Chow, B. W.-Y., & Shu, H. (2005). The role of morphological awareness in children's vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 415435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2001). Spoken word access processes: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 469490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meara, P. (1984). The study of lexis in interlanguage. In Davies, A., Criper, C., & Howatt, A. (Eds.), Interlanguage. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
Monsell, S. (1985). Repetition and the lexicon. In Ellis, A. W. (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Myers, J. (2007). Processing Chinese compounds: A survey of the literature. In Libben, G. & Jarema, G. (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 169196). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students, Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Variations in children's concepts by age and category. Child Development, 45, 577584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, B. A., & Uccelli, P. (2009). Semantic development: Learning the meanings of words. In Berko Gleason, J. & Bernsteain Ratner, N. (Eds.), The development of language (7th ed., pp. 104138). Toronto: Pearson.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21, 481504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandra, D. (1990). On the representation and processing of compound words: Automatic access to constituent morphemes does not occur. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42A, 529567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimomura, M. (1999). Kanji lexicality effect in partial repetition priming: The relationship between Kanji word and Kanji character processing. Brain and Language, 68, 8288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Zhang, H. C. (1995). Incidental learning of word meanings while reading: A Chinese and American cross-cultural study. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 7695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. Y. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Söderman, T. (1993). Word associations of foreign language learners and native speakers: The phenomenon of a shift in response type and its relevance for lexical development. In Ringbom, H. (Ed.), Near-native proficiency in English (pp. 91182). Åbo, Finland: Åbo Akademi, English Department Publications.Google Scholar
Sun, H. L., Huang, J. P., Sun, D. J., Li, D. J., & Xing, H. B. (1997). Introduction to language corpus system of modern Chinese study. In Hu, M. Y. (Ed.), Paper collection for the 5th World Chinese Teaching Symposium. Beijing: Peking University Publisher.Google Scholar
Sun, M. (2006). Balanced corpus of modern Chinese. Tsinghua, China: Tsinghua University AI and NLP Group. Retrieved from http://www.nlp.csai.tsinghua.edu.cnGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1995). The representation of bound morphemes in the lexicon: A Chinese study. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 293316). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Zukowski, A. (2009). Putting words together: Morphology and syntax in the preschool years. In Berko Gleason, J. & Bernsteain Ratner, N. (Eds.), The development of language (7th ed., pp. 139191). Toronto: Pearson.Google ScholarPubMed
Tang, Y. (1995). 普 通 话 语 音 统 计 分 析 [Analysis of Mandarin phonology]. Cheng De Normal School Journal, 1.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vannest, J., & Boland, J. E. (1999). Lexical morphology and lexical access. Brain and Language, 68, 324332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yin, B. (1984). 汉 语 语 素 的 定 量 研 究, [Quantitative analysis of Chinese morphemes]. Chinese, 5.Google Scholar
Yuan, C. F., & Huang, C. N. (1998). 基 于 语 素 数 据 库 的 汉 语 语 素 及 构 词 研 究 [Chinese morphemes and compounds: A corpus study]. Teaching Chinese as a Second Language, 3.Google Scholar
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2000). Lexical representation of compound words: Cross-linguistic evidence. Psychologia, 43, 4766.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2006). Becoming literate in different languages: Similar problems, different solutions. Developmental Science, 9, 429436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwitserlood, P. (1994). The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 341368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar