Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T17:14:16.568Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Introduction: special issue on atypical development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2012

LETITIA R. NAIGLES
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
EDITH L. BAVIN
Affiliation:
La Trobe University

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychological Association (APA) (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; Volume IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A. & Sabo, H. (1988). Dissociation between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndrome. In Bishop, E. & Mogford, K. (eds), Language development in exceptional circumstances, 177–89. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Brock, J., Einav, S. & Riby, D. (2008). The other end of the spectrum? Social cognition in Williams syndrome. In Striano, T. & Reid, V. (eds), Social cognition: Development, neuroscience and autism, 281300. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Cox, A., Baird, G. & Drew, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive development disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 38(3), 265–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (eds)1990. Children with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eden, G., Jones, K., Cappell, K., Gareau, L., Wood, F., Zeffiro, T., Dietz, N., Agnew, J. & Flowers, D. L. (2004). Neural changes following remediation in adult developmental dyslexia. Neuron 44, 411–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eigsti, I. M., Bennetto, L. & Dadlani, M. B. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37(6), 10071023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C. & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order. Psychological Science 17, 684–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. (1984). Biological predispositions to learn language. In Marler, P. & Terrace, T. (eds), The biology of learning, 553–84. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2004). The resilience of language. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, A., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. (2012) Comprehension of wh-questions precedes their production in typical development and autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research 5, 109123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. & Goldin Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16, 368–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 418–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the study of developmental disorders. Developmental Psychology 45, 5663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, Y. & Ebstein, R. (2009). Crossing syndrome boundaries in the search for brain endophenotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50(6), 657–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lum, J. A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D. & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex 48, 1138–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayo, J. & Eigsti, I. M. (in press). A comparison of statistical learning in school-aged children with high functioning autism and typically developing peers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R. (2002). Form is easy, meaning is hard: Resolving a paradox in early child language. Cognition 86, 157–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: William Morrow & Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M., Wexler, K. & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 38, 850–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 358(1430), 303314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1992). The social bases of language development. Social Development 1, 6787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. & Logrip, M. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying online sentence processing in young children. Cognition 73, 89134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition 92, 231–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M. T. & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex 41, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar