Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T14:59:24.764Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Which is important for preschoolers' production and repair of statements: What the listener knows or what the listener says?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 December 2011

ELIZABETH S. NILSEN*
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo
LEILANI MANGAL
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo
*
Address for correspondence: Elizabeth Nilsen, Psychology Department, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1. tel: 519-888-4567 x33991; fax: 519-746-8631; e-mail: enilsen@uwaterloo.ca

Abstract

Three- and four-year-olds participated in a referential communication task wherein they requested stickers from a knowledgeable or ignorant adult to complete a card. Following inadequate initial requests children were provided with three different feedback types: goal substitution (i.e. an incorrect sticker was provided), explicit statement of misunderstanding (‘I don't know which one you mean’), and vague feedback (‘Huh?’). Preschoolers' initial statements revealed sensitivity to the listener's perspective: more descriptors were provided when the listener did not have visual access to the card. Although listener's knowledge did not affect children's repair statements following feedback, the feedback type did: goal substitution elicited more repairs that included new descriptors, whereas vague responses elicited more repetition of initial requests than other feedback types. Children's age and verbal skills were related to the specific repair strategies used. Results demonstrate that preschoolers' use of cues from a conversational partner depends on the type of communicative task.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council Standard Research Grant awarded to EN. We thank the students and staff at the Early Childhood Education Centre at the University of Waterloo, as well as parents and children from the community, who participated in the study. The authors also thank Melissa Grubb for her assistance with data collection.

References

REFERENCES

Abbott and Costello (1945) Who's on First? The Naughty Nineties (1945). Retrieved 10 September 2010, from www.baseball-almanac.com/humor4.shtml.Google Scholar
Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M. & Acunzo, M. (1986). Young children's responses to neutral and specific contingent queries. Journal of Child Language 13, 135–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahtiyar, S. & Kuntay, A. C. (2009). Integration of communicative partner's visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language 36, 529–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coon, R. C., Lipscomb, T. J. & Copple, C. E. (1982). Effects of listener feedback on the messages of kindergarten children in a referential communication task. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 3, 337–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, W. & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition 11, 159–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickson, W. P. (1982). Two decades of referential communication research: A review and meta-analysis. In Brainerd, C. J. & Pressley, M. (eds), Verbal processes in children, 133. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Fagan, M. K. (2008). Toddlers' persistence when communication fails: Response motivation and goal substitution. First Language 28, 5569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S. & Krauss, R. (1967). What do people say after they have learned to talk? Studies of the development of referential communication. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly 13, 309316.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). ‘I beg your pardon?’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language 13, 455–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science 10, F1F7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition 108, 732–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, P., Mann, S. & Peers, I. (1998). The growth of speaker and listener skills from five to eleven years. First Language 18, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development 78, 1744–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menig-Peterson, C. L. (1975). The modification of communicative behaviour in preschool-aged children as a function of the listener's perspective. Child Development 46, 1015–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W. & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development 78, 622–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadig, A. S. & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science 13, 329–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcomer, P. L. & Hammill, D. D. (1997). Test of Language Development – Primary, 3rd edn.Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Nilsen, E. S. & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between children's communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology 58, 220–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development 67, 659–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, D. K. & Topolovec, J. (2001). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to the referential (in)efficacy of their own pointing gestures. Journal of Child Language 28, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Anselmi, D. & Farrar, M. J. (1984/1985). Young children's coordination of gestural and linguistic reference. First Language 5, 199209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, M. J. & Webster, E. J. (1980). Early discourse behaviour: An analysis of children's responses to listener feedback. Child Development 51, 1120–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittek, A. & Tomasello, M. (2005). Young children's sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics 26, 541–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar