Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T20:20:15.873Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of language skills in learning to read: The case of bilingualism in French overseas departments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 October 2011

ISABELLE NEGRO*
Affiliation:
Université Nice-Sophia Antipolis
SOPHIE GENELOT
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Isabelle Negro, Université Nice-Sophia Antipolis, Pôle Universitaire St Jean d'Angély, 24 Avenue des Diables Bleus, 06357 Nice Cedex 4, France. E-mail: Isabelle.Negro@unice.fr

Abstract

This study aims to explain how the practice of two languages (French and Creole) in French overseas departments affects the first educational competencies acquired by children. The students’ performance in both languages was investigated at the beginning of kindergarten, and their reading capacities were measured at the end of Grade 1. The data analysis shows that the practice of Creole has no negative impact on success at reading in French. Furthermore, it appears that the students who performed the best in reading were those who were either more competent in French than in Creole, or those who were equally competent in both languages, according to their assessed reading competence. Thus, also discussed is the necessity of early exposure to a language's written code and the contribution that bilingualism makes to learning processes of reading.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Akinci, M.-A., & Jisa, H. (2001). Développement de la narration en langue faible et forte: Le cas des connecteurs. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 14, 87110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beals, D. E., & Tabors, P. O. (1995). Arboretum, breaucratic, and carbohydrates: Preschoolers’ exposure to rare vocabulary at home. First Language, 15, 5776.Google Scholar
Ben Fadhel, S. E. (1998). Contexte éducatif socio-familial et acquisition de l'écrit à l'école élémentaire. Revue Française de Pédagogie, 124, 6980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, B. (1975). Langage et classes sociales. Paris: Editions Minuit.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 560567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Herman, J. (1999). Does bilingualism matter in early literacy? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 3544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilinguisme, biliteracy, and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 4361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 580590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, R. (2000). Acquisition de la phonologie et bilinguisme précoce. In Kail, M. & Fayol, M. (Eds.), L'acquisition du langage: Le langage en émergence de la naissance à 3 ans (Vol. 1, pp. 169192). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijeljac, R., & Breton, R. (1997). Du langage aux langues. Paris: Editions Gallimard Découverte.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1991). Developmental reading disabilities: The role of phonological processing has been overemphasized. Mind and Language, 6, 97101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 476487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L. (1990). Rhymes, language and children's reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 237252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, S., Espinosa, L., & Snow, C. E. (2003). Débuts de la littératie, langue et culture: Perspective socioculturelle. Revue des Sciences de l'Education, 29, 75100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, R., & Sais, E. (1995). Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 6168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalard, M., Bonin, P., Meot, A., Boyer, B., & Fayol, M. (2003). Objective age-of-acquisition (AoA) norms for a set of 230 object names in French: Relationships with psycholinguistic variables, the English data from Morrison et al. (1997), and naming latencies. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 15, 209245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Ku, Y.-M., Koyama, E., Anderson, R. C., & Li, W. (2008). Development of phonological awareness in bilingual Chinese children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37, 405418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Schatschneider, C. (2002). The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 399416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California State Department of Education (Dir.), Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework (pp. 349). Los Angeles: California State University, Evaluation, Dissemination and Assessment Center.Google Scholar
De Bot, K., & Schreuder, R. (1993). Word production and the bilingual lexicon. In Schreuder, R. & Weltens, B. (Eds.), The bilingual lexicon (pp. 191214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E., & Poe, M. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Clark-Chiarelli, N., & Wolf, A. (2004). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness in low-income Spanish and English bilingual preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 323347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dieterich, S. E., Assel, M. A., Swank, P., Smith, K. E., & Landry, S. H. (2006). The impact of early maternal verbal scaffolding and child language abilities on later decoding and reading comprehension skills. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 481494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. G., Colé, P., Seymour, P. H. K., & Magnan, A. (2006). Differing sequences of metaphonological development in French and English. Journal of Child Language, 33, 369399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (1997). How important are rhyme and analogy in beginning reading. Cognition, 63, 171208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duru-Bellat, M., & Van Zanten, A. (1999), Sociologie de l'école. Paris: Editions Armand Colin.Google Scholar
Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A. (2002). The development of epiphonologocal and metaphonological processing at the start of reading: A longitudinal study. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 17, 4762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1991). Learning to read and spell words. In Rieben, L. & Perfetti, C. (Eds.), Learning to read: Basic Research and its implications (pp. 5773). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read. Evidence from the National Reading Panel's meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 6575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Facthum-Sainton, J. (2006). Les langues créoles à base lexicale française de la Caraïbe: Phonétique, phonologie et variation. Thèse de doctorat. Paris: École Pratique des Hautes Études.Google Scholar
Foulin, J.-N. (2005). Why is letter–name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and Writing, 18, 129155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K., Marshall, J. C. & Coltearth, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301330). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Genelot, S. (2005). L'école à la Martinique, état des lieux. In Tupin, F. (Ed.), Ecoles ultramarines. Univers Créoles 5 (pp. 322). Paris: Anthropos, Economica.Google Scholar
Genelot, S., & Negro, I. (in press). Contribution à l'analyse des inégalités territoriales de réussite à l'école primaire. Performances langagières et cognitives d'élèves de GS en Martinique et en France métropolitaine. Revue Française de Pédagogie.Google Scholar
Genelot, S., & Negro, I. (2007). Bilinguisme familial et acquisitions scolaires: Le cas des départements d'outre-mer français (DOM). In Marquet, P., Hedjerassi, N., Jarlegan, A., Pacurar, E., & Remoussenard, E. (Eds.), Actualité de la Recherche en Education et Formation [CD].Google Scholar
Genelot, S., Negro, I., & Peslages, D. (2005). Compétences bilingues Français/Créole chez des enfants de 5 ans en contexte martiniquais. Etudes Créoles, 28, 4166.Google Scholar
Georger, F. (2005). Le réunionnais au secours du créole et du français à l'école. Etudes Créoles, 28, 119146.Google Scholar
Giraud, M., Gani, L., & Manesse, D. (1992). L'école aux Antilles: Langues et échec scolaire. Paris: Editions Karthala.Google Scholar
Gombert, J. E. (1990). Le développement des capacités métalinguistiques. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Gombert, J.-E. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning to read: Implication as for subtypes of dyslexia. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain and Cognition, 10. Retrieved from http://cpl.revues.org/index202.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gombert, J.-E. (2006). Epi/méta vs. implicite/explicite: Niveau de contrôle cognitif sur les traitements et apprentissage de la lecture. Langages et Pratiques, 38, 6876.Google Scholar
Gombert, J.-E., & Desvignes, J. (2002). Lecture. In Colmant, M., Jeantheau, J.-P., & Murat, F. (Eds.), Les compétences des élèves à l'entrée au CP. Etudes réalisées à partir du panel d'écoliers recrutés en 1997: Les dossiers (Vol. 132, pp. 5463). Paris: Ministère de l'Education Nationale et de la Recherche.Google Scholar
Gottardo, A. (2002). The relationship between language and reading skills in bilingual Spanish–English speakers. Topics in Language Disorders, 22, 4670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hazaël-Massieux, M. C. (2002). Les créoles à base française: Une introduction. Travaux Interdisciplinaires du Laboratoire Parole et Langage, 21, 6386.Google Scholar
Kail, M. (1983). L'acquisition du langage repensée: Les recherches interlangues. L'Année Psychologique, 83, 225258, 561–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katzir, T., Lesaux, N. K., & Kim, Y.-S. (2009). The role of reading self-concept and home literacy practices in fourth grade reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 22, 261276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kontos, S., & Wilcox-Herzog, S. (1997). Teacher's interaction with children: Why are they so important? Young Children, 52, 412.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesaux, N., Koda, K., Siegel, L., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Development of literacy: Meta analysis. In August, D. & Shanahan, T. (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language—Minority Children and Youth (pp. 75122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). Manulex: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary-school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 156166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonigan, C. J., Anthony, J. L., Phillips, B. M., Purpura, D. J., Wilson, S. B., & McQueen, J. D. (2009). The nature of preschool phonological processing abilities and their relations to vocabulary, general cognitive abilities, and print knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 345358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
March, C. (1996). Le discours des mères martiniquaises: Diglossie et créolité, un point de vue socio-linguistique. Paris: Editions L'harmattan.Google Scholar
Marian, V., Spivey, M., & Hirsch, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eye-tracking and brain imaging. Brain and Language, 86, 7082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Boukrina, O. V. (2008). Sensitivity to phonological similarity within and across languages. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37, 141170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and non-word repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-schools students. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 98, 134147.Google Scholar
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 85101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Negro, I., & Genelot, S. (2009). Les prédicteurs en grande section maternelle de la réussite en lecture en fin de première année de l'école primaire: L'impact du nom des lettres. Bulletin de Psychologie, 62, 291306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Negro, I., Genelot, S., & Peslages, D. (2005). Interaction between family languages and languages of schooling: The case of French and Creole in Martinique kindergartners. Paper presented at the EARLI Biennial Conference, University of Cyprus.Google Scholar
Negro, I., Genelot, S., & Peslages, D. (2006). Compétences langagières acquises en milieu familial et acquisitions scolaires en cycle 2: Le cas du créole et du français en contexte martiniquais. Rapport de recherche. ACI Ecole et Sciences Cognitives, Ministère délégué à la recherche et aux nouvelles technologies.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41, 428442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payet, J.-P., & van Zanten, A. (1996). L'école, les enfants de l'immigration et des minorités ethniques. Une revue de la littérature française, américaine et britannique. Revue Française de Pédagogie, 117, 87149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perdue, C., & Gaonac'h, D. (2000). Acquisition des langues secondes. In Kail, M. & Fayol, M. (Eds.), L'acquisition du langage: Le langage en développement au delà de 3 ans (Vol. 2, pp. 215246). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prêteur, Y. (1998). Education familiale et développement du rapport à l'écrit et à l'école. Les Dossiers d'Education et Formations, 101, 6992.Google Scholar
Prêteur, Y., & Rouquette, L. (1992). L'incidence d'une pédagogie fonctionnelle autour du livre de jeunesse sur les compétences orales et écrites d'enfants de GSM de milieux sociaux contrastés. Revue Française de Pédagogie, 98, 2940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prudent, L. F. (2005). L'école martiniquaise à la recherche de sa cohérence. In Tupin, F. (Ed.), Ecole ultramarine, Univers Créoles (Vol. 5, pp. 2346). Paris: Editions Economica Anthropos.Google Scholar
Reese, E., Suggate, S., Long, J., & Schaughency, E. (2009). Children's oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction. Reading and Writing, 23, 627644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savolainen, H., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Tolvanen, A., & Holopainen, L. (2008). Reading comprehension, word reading and spelling as predictors of school achievement and choice of secondary education. Learning and Instruction, 18, 201210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In Shapiro, B. K., Accardo, P. J., & Capute, A. J. (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 77121). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory and practice. In Neuman, S. B. & Dickinson, D. K. (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97110). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Silverberg, S., & Samuel, A. G. (2004). The effect of age of second language acquisition on the representation and processing of second language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 381398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Si Moussa, A. (2001). Evaluation et analyse des acquisitions des élèves en CE1 et CM1. Rapport de recherche “Charte pour l'école du XXIème siècle:” Mission Ecole primaire. Paris: INRP.Google Scholar
Si Moussa, A. (2005). Evaluation et déterminants de la réussite scolaire. In Si Moussa, A. (Eds.), L'école à la Réunion, approches plurielles (pp. 5184). Paris: Karthala.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Speece, D. L., Roth, F. A., Cooper, D. H., & de la Paz, S. (1999). The relevance of oral language skills to early literacy: A multivariate analysis. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 167190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, S. S. (2003). Vocabulary and readability: How knowing word meanings affects comprehension. Topics in Language Disorders, 23, 241247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L., Rosston, K., Gerber, M., & Solari, E. (2008). Influence of oral language and phonological awareness on children's bilingual reading. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 413429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhoeven, L. T. (2000). Components in early second language reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 313330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-McGregor, M., & Leung, Y. K. I. (2004). Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winskel, H., & Widjaja, V. (2007). Phonological awareness, letter development, and literacy development in Indonesian beginner readers and spellers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 2345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar