Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T14:46:07.080Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is retrieval-induced forgetting behind the bilingual disadvantage in word production?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 August 2011

ELIN RUNNQVIST
Affiliation:
Universitat de Barcelona, Departamento de Psicologia Bàsica, Barcelona, Cataluña, Spain
ALBERT COSTA*
Affiliation:
Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Departamento de Tecnologia, ICREA, Barcelona, Cataluña, Spain
*
Address for correspondence: Albert Costa, Departamento de Tecnologia, ICREA, Universitat de Pompeu Fabra, C/Tanger, 122–140, 08018 Barcelona, Spaincostalbert@gmail.com

Abstract

Levy, Mc Veigh, Marful and Andreson (2007) found that naming pictures in L2 impaired subsequent recall of the L1 translation words. This was interpreted as evidence for a domain-general inhibitory mechanism (RIF) underlying first language attrition. Because this result is at odds with some previous findings and theoretical assumptions, we wanted to assess its reliability and replicate the experiment with various groups. Participants were first shown drawings along with their labels in the non-dominant language. Afterwards, they named 75% of these drawings in their first language or in their non-dominant language. Finally, participants’ memory of all L1 words was tested through the presentation of a rhyme-cue. Recall of L1 words was better after naming pictures in the non-dominant language compared to when the picture was not named at all. This result suggests that speaking a second language protects rather than harms the memory of our first language.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by three grants from the Spanish Government (SEJ 2005/SEJ2005–01223/CONSOLIDER-INGENIO) and grants PSIC, PSI2008-00464/PSIC and SGR “2009–1521”. Elin Runnqvist was supported by a Pre-doctoral fellowship from the Catalan Government (FI). The authors are grateful to Kristof Strijkers, Dr. Clara Martin, Dr. David Green, Dr. René Zeelenberg and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on previous versions of this manuscript.

References

Anderson, M. C., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 522530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M. C., & McCulloch, K. C. (1999). Integration as a general boundary condition on retrieval-induced forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 608629.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. C., Green, C., & McCulloch, K. C. (2000). Similarity and inhibition in long-term memory: Evidence for a two-factor model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 11411159.Google Scholar
Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102, 68100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnhardt, T. M., Glisky, E. L., Polster, M. R., & Elam, L. (1996). Inhibition of associates and activation of synonyms in the rare-word paradigm: Further evidence for a center-surround mechanism. Memory & Cognition, 24, 6069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauml, K., & Hartinger, A. (2002). On the role of item similarity in retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory, 10, 215224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859873.Google ScholarPubMed
Camp, G., Pecher, D., & Schmidt, H. G. (2007) No retrieval-induced forgetting using item-specific independent cues: Evidence against a general inhibitory account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (5), 950958.Google ScholarPubMed
Carr, T. H., & Dagenbach, D. C. (1990). Semantic priming and repetition priming from masked words: Evidence for a center-surround attentional mechanism in perceptual recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 341350.Google ScholarPubMed
Chan, J. C. K. (2009). When does retrieval induce forgetting and when does it induce facilitation? Implications for retrieval inhibition, testing effect, and text processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 153170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, J. C. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. III (2006). Retrieval-induced facilitation: Initially nontested material can benefit from prior testing of related material. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 533571.Google ScholarPubMed
Ciranni, M. A., & Shimamura, A. P. (1999). Retrieval-induced forgetting in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 14031414Google ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish–English and English–Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2 (3), 231244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual's two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 365397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, W. S., Augustini, B. K., & Saenz, S. P. (2003). Repetition priming in picture naming and translation depends on shared processes and their difficulty: Evidence from Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 12831297.Google ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T. H., Fennema-Notestine, C., Montoya, R. I., & Jernigan, T. L. (2007). The bilingual effect on Boston Naming Test performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 197208.Google ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K. (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33, 12201234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. (2002). Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish–English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. W. (1986). Control, activation and resource. Brain and Language, 27, 210223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (2), 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (1998). Constraint, word frequency, and the relationship between lexical processing levels in spoken word production. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 313338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127, 277288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köpke, B. (2007). Language attrition at the crossroads of brain, mind, and society. In Köpke, B., Schmid, M. S., Keijzer, M. & Dostert, S. (eds.), Language attrition: Theoretical perspectives, 937. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpke, B., & Schmid, M. S. (2004). Language attrition: The next phase. In Schmid, M. S. Köpke, B. Keijzer, M. & Weilemar, L. (eds.), First language attrition: Interdisciplinary perspective on methodological issues, pp. 143. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, M. W., & Williams, J. N. (2001). Lexical access in spoken word production by bilinguals: Evidence from the semantic competitor priming paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4 (3), 233248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2002). Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 299305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. J., Mc Veigh, N., Marful, A., & Anderson, M. C. (2007). Inhibiting your native language: The role of retrieval-induced forgetting during second-language acquisition. Psychological Science, 18 (1), 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 2941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, M. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2001). Gone but not forgotten: the transient nature of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12, 148152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., & MacLeod, M. D. (1999). On recollections lost: When practice makes imperfect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 463473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., Costa, A., Peterson, R., Vargas, K. A., & Caramazza, A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture–word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 33, 503535.Google Scholar
Sandoval, T. C., Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., & Salmon, D. P. (2010). What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (2), 231252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S. (2002). First language attrition, use and maintenance: The case of German Jews in anglophone countries. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S. (2010). Languages at play: The relevance of L1 attrition to the study of bilingualism. In Schmid, M. S. (ed.), New perspectives on L1 attrition, Special Issue of Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (1), 17.Google Scholar
Seliger, H. W., & Vago, R. M. (1991). The study of first language attrition: An overview. In Seliger, H. W. & Vago, R. M. (eds.), First language attrition, pp. 316. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeelenberg, R., & Pecher, D. (2003). Evidence for long-term crosslanguage repetition priming in conceptual implicit memory tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 8094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar