Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T17:39:19.385Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of scaffolding and overhearing on young children's use of the spatial terms between and middle*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 July 2011

EMILY K. FOSTER
Affiliation:
Illinois State University
ALYCIA M. HUND*
Affiliation:
Illinois State University
*
Address for correspondence: amhund@ilstu.edu

Abstract

The primary goal was to specify the impact of scaffolding and overhearing on young children's use of the spatial terms between and middle. Four- and five-year-old children described the location of a mouse hidden between two furniture items in a dollhouse with assistance from a parent. Children's use of between and middle increased significantly across trials, and in concert, parents' directive scaffolding involving middle decreased across trials. In the second study, three common scaffolding types (Between Directive, Middle Directive, non-directive) were compared with a no prompt condition by having children receive prompts from a doll and with overhearing conditions in which children overheard conversations between two adult experimenters containing between or middle. Children's use of between and middle was much more frequent following directive prompting than following non-directive prompting, no prompting, or overhearing. Moreover, children showed some evidence of using between and middle in response to non-directive prompting and overhearing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We thank Gregory Braswell, Jeffrey Wagman, and the editors and reviewers for helpful comments on earlier drafts. We also thank Adam Craig, Kileigh Fleming, Kathleen Hines, Jenna Krause, Brittni Lauinger, Stacey McLellan, Chelsea Mueller, Cassidy Powell, Jayme Jones, Kate Swinden and Danielle Zimmerman for assistance with data collection, transcription and coding. We thank Advocate BroMenn Childcare Center by Rogy's, Alphabet Soup Academy, Blooming Grove Academy, Christ the King Community Preschool, Clubhouse Childcare Center, Cornerstone Christian Academy, Discovery World Childcare Center, Illinois State University Childcare Center, Katie's Kids Learning Centers, Little Jewels Learning Center, The Mulberry School, Rogy's Learning Place, Scribbles Center for Learning, Tomorrow's Promise Learning Center and Wesley Bright Beginnings, as well as all of the children and parents for their enthusiastic cooperation.

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science 8, 199209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, N., Jipson, J. & Callanan, M. A. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development 72, 416–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, T. K., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S. & Oh, J. S. (2002). Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science 13, 238–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mother's directives as children age. Journal of Child Language 6, 443–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M. & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning. Child Development 81, 326–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blades, M. & Cooke, Z. (1994). Young children's ability to understand a model as a spatial representation. Journal of Genetic Psychology 155, 201218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, J. G. & Idowu, T. C. (1987). Constructing favorable conditions for measuring the young child's understanding of the terms in, on, and under. International Journal of Behavioral Development 10, 8998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M. A. (1985). How parents label objects for young children: The role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development 56, 508523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M. A. (1989). Development of object categories and inclusion relations: Preschoolers' hypotheses about word meanings. Developmental Psychology 25, 207216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology 28, 595616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasola, M. (2005). Can language do the driving? The effect of linguistic input on infants' categorization of support spatial relations. Developmental Psychology 41, 183–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasola, M. (2008). The development of infants' spatial categories. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17, 2125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasola, M. & Cohen, L. B. (2002). Infant categorization of containment, support, and tight-fit spatial relationships. Developmental Science 5, 247–64.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings. Cognition 2, 161–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Correa-Chávez, M. & Rogoff, B. (2009). Children's attention to interactions directed to others: Guatemalan Mayan and European American patterns. Developmental Psychology 45, 630–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, R., Halpern, E., Aviezer, O. & Goldblatt, A. (1981). The development of three spatial concepts: In, on, under. International Journal of Behavioral Development 4, 403419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, M. V., Batra, P. & Singhal, S. (1981). A cross-cultural study of young children's understanding of spatial prepositions. International Journal of Behavioral Development 4, 469–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dromi, E. (1978). More on the acquisition of locative prepositions: An analysis of Hebrew data. Journal of Child Language 6, 547–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkin, K. (1981). Aspects of late language acquisition: School children's use and comprehension of prepositions. First Language 2, 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkin, K. (1983). Children's comprehension of complex sentences containing the preposition between. Child Study Journal 13, 133–47.Google Scholar
Erreich, A. & Valian, V. (1979). Children's internal organization of locative categories. Child Development 50, 1071–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floor, P. & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy 9, 327–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gauvain, M., Fagot, B. I., Leve, C. & Kavanagh, K. (2002). Instruction by mothers and fathers during problem solving with young children. Journal of Family Psychology 16, 8190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. & Rattermann, M. J. (1991). Language and the career of similarity. In Gelman, S. & Byrns, J. (eds), Perspectives on language and thought, 225–77. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, K. M., Kim, A. S. & Whiren, A. (2003). The effect of verbal scaffolding on the complexity of preschool children's block constructions. In Lytle, D. E. (ed.), Play and educational theory and practice, 117–33. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar
Halford, G. S. (1993). Children's understanding: The development of mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hughes, C. H. & Ensor, R. A. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? In Lewis, C. & Carpendale, J. L. M. (eds), Social interactions and development of executive function. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development 123, 3550.Google Scholar
Hund, A. M. (2010). What factors shape by ratings in relation to landmarks? Journal of Cognition and Development 11, 374–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hund, A. M. & Naroleski, A. R. (2008). Developmental changes in young children's spatial memory and language in relation to landmarks. Journal of Cognition and Development 9, 310–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hund, A. M. & Plumert, J. M. (2007). What counts as “by”? Young children's use of relative distance to judge nearbyness. Developmental Psychology 43, 121–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Internicola, R. W. & Weist, R. M. (2003). The acquisition of simple and complex spatial locatives in English: A longitudinal investigation. First Language 23, 239–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. & Landau, B. (1991). Spatial language and spatial cognition. In Napoli, D. J. & Kegl, J. A. (eds), Bridges between psychology and linguistics, 145–69. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R. (1981). On location: Thinking and talking about space. Topics in Language Disorders 2, 1731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. R. & Slobin, D. I. (1979). The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish. Journal of Child Language 6, 529–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kermani, H. & Brenner, M. E. (2000). Maternal scaffolding in the child's Zone of Proximal Development across tasks: Cross-cultural perspectives. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 15, 3052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B. (1996). Multiple geometric representations of objects in languages and language learners. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L. & Garrett, M. (eds), Language and space, 317–64. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S., Miller-Loncar, C., Smith, K. E. & Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children's development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology 21, 1541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, H. G. (1996). Context and scaffolding in developmental studies of mother–child problem-solving dyads. In Chaiklin, S. & Lave, J. (eds), Understanding practice: Perspectives on activity and context, 306326. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, J. & Gentner, D. (2005). Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Cognitive Psychology 50, 315–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G. D. & Sadler, D. D. (1996). A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M. A., Nadel, L. & Garrett, M. (eds), Language and space, 493529. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, A., Correa-Chávez, M., Rogoff, B. & Gutiérrez, K. (2010). Attention to instruction directed to another by U.S. Mexican-heritage children of varying cultural backgrounds. Developmental Psychology 46, 593601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez-Sussmann, C., Akhtar, N., Diesendruck, G. & Markson, L. (2011). Orienting to third-party conversations. Journal of Child Language 38, 273–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meints, K., Plunkett, K., Harris, P. L. & Dimmock, D. (2002). What is ‘on’ and ‘under’ for 15-, 18-, and 24-month-olds? Typicality effects in early comprehension of spatial prepositions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 20, 113–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messick, C. K. (1988). Ins and outs of the acquisition of spatial terms. Topics in Language Disorders 8, 1425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B. & Angelillo, C. (2003). Cultural variation in young children's access to work or involvement in specialized child-focused activities. International Journal of Behavioral Development 27, 264–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E. & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth order effects on early language development: Do second-born children learn from overheard speech? Child Development 67, 621–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumert, J. M., Ewert, K. & Spear, S. J. (1995). The early development of children's communication about nested spatial relations. Child Development 66, 959–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumert, J. M. & Hawkins, A. M. (2001). Biases in young children's communication about spatial relations: Containment versus proximity. Child Development 72, 2236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plumert, J. M. & Nichols-Whitehead, P. (1996). Parental scaffolding of young children's spatial communication. Developmental Psychology 32, 523–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, P. C. (1994). The categorization of above and below spatial relations by young infants. Child Development 65, 5869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, P. C., Adams, A., Kennedy, E., Shettler, L. & Wasnick, A. (2003). Development of an abstract category representation for the spatial relation between in 6- to 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 39, 151–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, P. C., Cummins, M., Kase, J., Martin, E. & Weissman, S. (1996). Development of categorical representations for above and below spatial relations in 3- to 7-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 32, 942–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, J. B., Burns, B. M. & Winders-Davis, D. (2009). Maternal scaffolding and attention regulation in children living in poverty. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 30, 8291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (eds), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language, 679744. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Ellis, S. & Gardner, W. (1984). Adjustment of adult–child instruction according to child's age and task. Developmental Psychology 20, 193–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (2007). Cognitive variability. Developmental Science 10, 104109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simms, N. & Gentner, D. (2008). Spatial language and landmark use: Can 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds find the middle? Paper presented at the annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Stevenson, D. L. & Baker, D. P. (1987). The family–school relation and the child's school performance. Child Development 58, 1348–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Washington, D. S. & Naremore, R. C. (1978). Children's use of spatial prepositions in two- and three-dimensional tasks. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 21, 151–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weist, R. M. & Lyytinen, P. (1991). The development of spatial location in Finnish. International Journal of Psychology 26, 345–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weist, R. M., Lyytinen, P., Wysocka, J. & Atanassova, M. (1997). The interaction of language and thought in children's language acquisition: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language 24, 81–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wertsch, J. V., McNamee, G. D., McLane, J. B. & Budwig, N. A. (1980). The adult–child dyad as a problem-solving system. Child Development 51, 1215–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, D., Bruner, J. & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry 17, 89–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control: II. The development of executive function in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science 7, 121–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar