Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-94d59 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T08:59:42.924Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Narrating psychological distress: Associations between cross-clausal integration and mental health difficulties

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2011

JÖRG ZINKEN*
Affiliation:
University of Portsmouth
CAROLINE BLAKEMORE
Affiliation:
University of Southampton
KATARZYNA ZINKEN
Affiliation:
Warsaw School of Social Sciences and Humanities
LISA BUTLER
Affiliation:
Portsmouth City Primary Care Trust
T. CHAS SKINNER
Affiliation:
Combined Universities Centre for Rural Health
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Jörg Zinken, Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, King Henry Building, King Henry I Street, Portsmouth PO1 2DY, UK. E-mail: joerg.zinken@port.ac.uk

Abstract

Psychological research has emphasized the importance of narrative for a person's sense of self. Building a coherent narrative of past events is one objective of psychotherapy. However, in guided self-help therapy the patient has to develop this narrative autonomously. Identifying patients’ narrative skills in relation to psychological distress could provide useful information about their suitability for self-help. The aim of this study was to explore whether the syntactic integration of clauses into narrative in texts written by prospective psychotherapy patients was related to mild to moderate psychological distress. Cross-clausal syntax of texts by 97 people who had contacted a primary care mental health service was analyzed. Severity of symptoms associated with mental health difficulties was assessed by a standardized scale (Clinical Outcomes in Routine Evaluation outcome measure). Cross-clausal syntactic integration was negatively correlated with the severity of symptoms. A multiple regression analysis confirmed that the use of simple sentences, finite complement clauses, and coordinated clauses was associated with symptoms (R2 = .26). The results suggest that the analysis of cross-clausal syntax can provide information on patients’ narrative skills in relation to distressing events and can therefore provide additional information to support treatment decisions.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bruner, J. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry, 18, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucci, W., & Freedman, N. (1981). The language of depression. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 45, 334358.Google ScholarPubMed
Croft, W. (1998). The structure of events and the structure of language. In Tomasello, M. (Ed.), The new psychology of language. Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure (pp. 6792). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Evans, C., Connell, J., Barkham, M., Margison, F., McGrath, G., Mellor-Clark, J., et al. (2002). Towards a standardised brief outcome measure: Psychometric properties and utility of the CORE-OM. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 5160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 1421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gellatly, J., Bower, P., Hennessy, S., Richards, D., Gilbody, S., & Lovell, K. (2007). What makes self-help interventions effective in the management of depressive symptoms? Meta-analysis and meta-regression. Psychological Medicine, 37, 12171228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghaderi, A. (2006). Attrition and outcome in self-help treatment for bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder: A constructive replication. Eating Behaviors, 7, 300308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labov, W. (1972). The transformation of experience in narrative syntax. Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black English vernacular. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites (Vol. 1).Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, I. (2004). Psychiatry in the future. Psychiatric Bulletin, 28, 319320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEnery, T., Tono, Y., & Xiao, R. Z. (2006). Corpus-based language studies: An advanced resource book. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
National Institute for Clinical Excellence. (2004). Depression: Management of depression in primary and secondary care. London: Author.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T. J., & Francis, M. E. (1997). Linguistic predictors of adaptive bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 863871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use. Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 12431254.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2004). Taking pen to hand: Evaluating theories underlying the written disclosure paradigm. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 121137.Google Scholar
Smyth, J. M., True, N., & Souto, J. (2001). Effects of writing about traumatic experiences: The necessity for narrative structuring. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 161172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallerman, M. (2005). Understanding syntax (2nd ed.). London: Hodder Arnold.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2007). A usage-based approach to child language acquisition. In Evans, V., Bergen, B., & Zinken, J. (Eds.), The cognitive linguistics reader (pp. 834848). London: Equinox.Google Scholar