Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-94d59 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T18:40:46.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parental numeric language input to Mandarin Chinese and English speaking preschool children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2010

ALICIA CHANG*
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
CATHERINE M. SANDHOFER
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
LAUREN ADELCHANOW
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
BENJAMIN ROTTMAN
Affiliation:
Yale University
*
Address for correspondence: Alicia Chang, Room 816, Learning Research and Development Center, 3939 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15260. tel: (412) 624-2679; e-mail: aliciac@pitt.edu

Abstract

The present study examined the number-specific parental language input to Mandarin- and English-speaking preschool-aged children. Mandarin and English transcripts from the CHILDES database were examined for amount of numeric speech, specific types of numeric speech and syntactic frames in which numeric speech appeared. The results showed that Mandarin-speaking parents talked about number more frequently than English-speaking parents. Further, the ways in which parents talked about number terms in the two languages was more supportive of a cardinal interpretation in Mandarin than in English. We discuss these results in terms of their implications for numerical understanding and later mathematical performance.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We thank the UCLA Language and Cognitive Development Lab and Barbara Sarnecka for helpful advice and comments. Alicia Chang is now at the University of Pittsburgh.

References

REFERENCES

Case, R. & Griffin, S. (1990). Child cognitive development: The role of central conceptual structures in the development of scientific and social thought. Advances in Psychology 64, 193230.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. L. S. & Sybesma, R. (1998). Yi-wan Tang, Yi-ge Tang: Classifiers and massifiers. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies, New Series 28(3), 385412.Google Scholar
Chien, Y., Lust, B. & Chiang, C. (2003). Chinese children's comprehension of count-classifiers and mass-classifiers. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 12, 91–120.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (2007). Commentary on Le Corre & Carey. Cognition 105, 439–45.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1994). Children's mathematical development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., Fan, L. & Siegler, R. S. (1993). Even before formal instruction, Chinese children outperform American children in mental addition. Cognitive Development 8(4), 517–29.Google Scholar
Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child's understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 236–48.Google Scholar
Jordan, N. C., Levine, S. C. & Huttenlocher, J. (1995). Calculation abilities in young children with different patterns of cognitive functioning. Journal of Learning Disabilities 28(1), 5364.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. (1990). The Child Language Data Exchange System: An update. Journal of Child Language 17, 457–72.Google Scholar
Milgram, R. J. (2004). What is mathematical proficiency? In Schoenfeld, A. H. (ed.), Assessing mathematical proficiency, 3158. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, K. F., Major, S. M., Shu, H. & Zhang, H. (2000). Ordinal knowledge: Number names and number concepts in Chinese and English. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 54(2), 129–40.Google Scholar
Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J. & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science 6(1), 5660.Google Scholar
Mintz, T. H. & Gleitman, L. R. (2002). Modifiers really do modify nouns: The incremental and restricted nature of early modifier acquisition. Cognition 84, 267–93.Google Scholar
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J. & Chrostowski, S. J. (2004). Findings from IEA's Trends in International Mathematics Science Study at the Fourth and Eighth Grades. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College.Google Scholar
Pagani, L. S., Jalbert, J. & Girard, A. (2006). Does preschool enrichment of precursors to arithmetic influence intuitive knowledge of number in low income children? Early Childhood Education Journal 34(2), 133–46.Google Scholar
Sarnecka, B. W., Kamenskaya, V. G., Yamana, Y., Ogura, T. & Yudovina, J. B. (2007). From grammatical number to exact numbers: Early meanings of ‘one’, ‘two’, and ‘three’ in English, Russian, and Japanese. Cognitive Psychology 55, 136–68.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. & Mu, Y. (2008). Chinese children excel on novel mathematics problems even before elementary school. Psychological Science 19(8), 759–63.Google Scholar
Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S.-Y. & Stigler, J. W. (1986). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American Children. Science 231(4739), 693–99.Google Scholar
Stevenson, H. W. & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap: Why our schools are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chinese education. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Suriyakham, L. W., Levine, S. C. & Huttenlocher, J. (2006). A naturalistic study of input effects on the development of number concepts. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 2613.Google Scholar
Vermeer, A. (2001). Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input. Applied Psycholinguistics 22(2), 217–34.Google Scholar

CHILDES CORPORA REFERENCES

Tardif, T. (1993). Adult-to-child speech and language acquisition in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32, 492504.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. (1993). Adult-to-child speech and language acquisition in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32, 492504.Google Scholar

English – US

Bates, E., Bretherton, I. & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, N. (1982). Acoustic study of mothers' speech to language-learning children: An analysis of vowel articulatory characteristics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: The use of single-word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Carlson-Luden, V. (1979). Causal understanding in the 10-month-old. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado at Boulder.Google Scholar
Demetras, M. (1989a). Changes in parents' conversational responses: A function of grammatical development. Paper presented at ASHA, St. Louis, MO.Google Scholar
Demetras, M. (1989b). Working parents' conversational responses to their two-year-old sons. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Demetras, M., Post, K. & Snow, C. (1986). Feedback to first-language learners. Journal of Child Language 13, 275–92.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B. (1980). The acquisition of social speech and politeness formulae. In Giles, H., Robinson, W. P. & Smith, P. M. (eds), Language: Social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Higginson, R. P. (1985). Fixing-assimilation in language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Washington State University.Google Scholar
Post, K. (1992). The language learning environment of laterborns in a rural Florida community. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Post, K. (1994). Negative evidence. In Sokolov, J. & Snow, C. (eds), Handbook of research in language development using CHILDES, 132–73. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent–child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, Vol. 4, 128. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Suppes, P. (1974). The semantics of children's language. American Psychologist 29, 103114.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition 40, 2181.Google Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A. (1982). Sex differences in speech to children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgia Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Warren-Leubecker, A. & Bohannon, J. N. (1984). Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother–father speech. Child Development 55, 1379–85.Google Scholar

English – UK

Wells, C. G. (1981). Learning through interaction: The study of language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar