Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-xxrs7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T09:45:26.701Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Orienting to third-party conversations*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 May 2010

CARMEN MARTÍNEZ-SUSSMANN*
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
NAMEERA AKHTAR
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
GIL DIESENDRUCK
Affiliation:
Bar Ilan University
LORI MARKSON
Affiliation:
Washington University
*
Address for correspondence: Carmen Martínez-Sussmann, Psychology Department, University of California Santa Cruz, 1156 High Street, Santa Cruz, CA 95064. e-mail: olimpia1@ucsc.edu

Abstract

Children as young as two years of age are able to learn novel object labels through overhearing, even when distracted by an attractive toy (Akhtar, 2005). The present studies varied the information provided about novel objects and examined which elements (i.e. novel versus neutral information and labels versus facts) toddlers chose to monitor, and what type of information they were more likely to learn. In Study 1, participants learned only the novel label and the novel fact containing a novel label. In Study 2, only girls learned the novel label. Neither girls nor boys learned the novel fact. In both studies, analyses of children's gaze patterns suggest that children who learned the new information strategically oriented to the third-party conversation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We would like to thank all the families that participated and the following research assistants who helped with various aspects of data collection and coding: Lauren DiMaggio, Justin Henderson, Lily Hendlin, Rubina Jetley, Lyssa Kaehler, Alexandra Koenig, Ronit Moreh, Yumiko Nakai, Jessica Polk-Berkowitz, Marisa Raditsch, Nicol Ruber, Katie Scherrman, Alex Smookler, Rebecca Spence, Melissa Sussmann, Tiffany Teh, Kristin Weisler and Hannah Williams. Special thanks to Barbara Rogoff for helpful comments on a previous version of the manuscript. This research was supported by a National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Pre-doctoral Training Grant (9 T32 HD046423-07) to the first author and faculty research funds granted by the University of California, Santa Cruz to the second author.

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N. (2004). Contexts of early word learning. In Hall, D. G. & Waxman, S. (eds), Weaving a lexicon, 485507. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science 8, 199209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, N., Jipson, J. & Callanan, M. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development 72, 416–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial No. 238, 58, 1165.Google Scholar
Barton, M. E. & Tomasello, M. (1991). Joint attention and conversation in mother–infant–sibling triads. Child Development 62, 517–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (eds), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language, 309370. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, P. (1998). Conversational structure and language acquisition: The role of repetition in Tzeltal. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 8, 197221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Munakata, Y. & Johnson, M. H. (eds), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and performance XXI, 249–74. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de León, L. (1998). The emergent participant: Interactive patterns in the socialization of Tzotzil (Mayan) infants. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 8, 131–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, G. & Markson, L. (2001). Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology 37, 630–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J. & Shatz, M. (1989). Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having an older sibling. Child Development 60, 399410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J. & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59(5, Serial No. 242).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S. & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics 21, 95–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Floor, P. & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy 9, 327–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herold, K. H. & Akhtar, N. (2008). Imitative learning from a third-party interaction: Relations with self-recognition and perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101, 114–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, M. A. & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development 76, 1261–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, E. V. M. (1994). Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds), Input and interaction in language acquisition, 5673. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markson, L. & Bloom, P. (1997). Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature 385, 813–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W. E., Marazita, J. & Lipko, A. R. (2009). How young children decide whether a word is one they know: Evidence for a gender-related difference. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B. & Angelillo, C. (2003). Cultural variation in young children's access to work or involvement in specialized child-focused activities. International Journal of Behavioral Development 27, 264–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Shweder, R. & LeVine, R. (eds), Culture theory: Mind, self, and emotion, 276320. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y. (1988). Children learn from speech not addressed to them: The case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language 15, 95–108.Google Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E. & Derevensky, J. (1996). Birth order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Development 67, 621–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oviatt, S. L. (1985). Tracing developmental change in language comprehension ability before twelve months of age. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 24, 8794.Google Scholar
Reznick, J. S. & Goldfield, B. A. (1992). Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology 28, 406413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Paradise, R., Mejia Arauz, R., Correa-Chavez, M. & Angelillo, C. (2003). Firsthand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology 54, 175203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization across cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, R., Behrend, D. & Ransom, A. (2009). Learning words and facts through overhearing. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, 675735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trehub, S. E. & Shenfield, T. (2007). Acquisition of early words from single-word and sentential contexts. Developmental Science 10, 190–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson-Gegeo, K. A. & Gegeo, D. W. (1986). Calling-out and repeating routines in Kwara'ae children's language socialization. In Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (eds), Language socialization across cultures, 1750. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar