Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T15:37:28.084Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Underspecification and asymmetries in voicing perception*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 July 2010

So-One K. Hwang
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
Philip J. Monahan
Affiliation:
Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language
William J. Idsardi
Affiliation:
University of Maryland

Abstract

The purpose of our study is to show that phonological knowledge is an important basis for making predictions during speech perception. Taking the phonological constraint in English that coda obstruent clusters agree in their value for voicing, we conducted two experiments using vowel–stop–fricative sequences, where the task was to identify the fricative. Stimuli included sequences that were either congruent or incongruent. Consistent with models of featural underspecification for voiceless obstruents, our results indicate that only voiced stops induced predictions for an upcoming voiced fricative, eliciting processing difficulty when such predictions were not met. In contrast, voiceless stops appear to induce no equivalent predictions. These results demonstrate the important role of abstract phonological knowledge in online processing, and the asymmetries in our findings also suggest that only specified features are the basis for generating perceptual predictions about the upcoming speech signal.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Archangeli, Diana (1988). Aspects of underspecification theory. Phonology 5. 183207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avery, Peter (1996). The representation of voicing contrasts. PhD dissertation, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Avery, Peter & Idsardi, William J. (2001). Laryngeal dimensions, completion and enhancement. In Hall (2001). 4170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avery, Peter & Rice, Keren (1989). Segment structure and coronal underspecification. Phonology 6. 179200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berent, Iris, Lennertz, Tracy, Jun, Jongho, Moreno, Miguel A. & Smolensky, Paul (2008). Language universals in human brains. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105. 53215325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David (2008). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (version 5.0.08). http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1988). Toward a substantive theory of feature specification. NELS 18. 7993.Google Scholar
Crawley, Michael J. (2007). The R book. Chichester: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dresher, B. Elan, Piggott, Glyne & Rice, Keren (1995). Contrast in phonology: overview. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 13. iiixvii.Google Scholar
Eulitz, Carsten & Lahiri, Aditi (2004). Neurobiological evidence for abstract phonological representations in the mental lexicon during speech recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16. 577583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flagg, Elissa J., Oram Cardy, Janis E. & Roberts, Timothy P. L. (2006). MEG detects neural consequences of anomalous nasalization in vowel–consonant pairs. Neuroscience Letters 397. 263268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, Carol A. & Brown, Julie M. (2000). Perceptual parsing of acoustic consequences of velum lowering from information for vowels. Perception and Psychophysics 62. 2132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedrich, Claudia K., Eulitz, Carsten & Lahiri, Aditi (2006). Not every pseudoword disrupts word recognition: an ERP study. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2:36.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Joseph H. (1978). Some generalisations concerning initial and final consonant clusters. In Greenberg, Joseph H. (ed.) Universals of human languages. Vol. 2: Phonology. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 243279.Google Scholar
Hall, T. Alan (ed.) (2001). Distinctive feature theory. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halle, Morris (2002). From memory to speech and back: papers on phonetics and phonology 1954–2002. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Halle, Morris & Stevens, Kenneth N. (1962). Speech recognition: a model and a program for research. IRE Transactions on Information Theory 8. 155159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harms, Robert T. (1978). Some nonrules of English. In Jazayery, Mohammad Ali, Polomé, Edgar C. & Winter, Werner (eds.) Linguistic and literary studies in honor of Archibald A. Hill. Vol. 2: Descriptive linguistics. The Hague: Mouton. 3951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, So-One K., Monahan, Philip J. & Idsardi, William J. (2009). Asymmetric phonological predictions in speech perception: MEG evidence. Paper presented at the 16th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Keating, Patricia A. (1988). Underspecification in phonetics. Phonology 5. 275292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreitman, Rina (2007). The phonetics and phonology of onset clusters: the case of Modern Hebrew. PhD dissertation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Lahiri, Aditi & Marslen-Wilson, William (1991). The mental representation of lexical form: a phonological approach to the recognition lexicon. Cognition 38. 245294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahiri, Aditi & Reetz, Henning (2002). Underspecified recognition. In Gussenhoven, Carlos & Warner, Natasha (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 637675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahiri, Aditi & Reetz, Henning (2010). Distinctive features: phonological underspecification in representation and processing. JPh 38. 4459.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74. 431461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombardi, Linda (1991). Laryngeal features and laryngeal neutralization. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Lombardi, Linda (1995). Dahl's law and privative [voice]. LI 26. 365372.Google Scholar
Lombardi, Linda (1999). Positional faithfulness and voicing assimilation in Optimality Theory. NLLT 17. 267302.Google Scholar
Massaro, Dominic W. & Cohen, Michael M. (1983). Phonological context in speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics 34. 338348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mester, Armin & Itô, Junko (1989). Feature predictability and underspecification: palatal prosody in Japanese mimetics. Lg 65. 258293.Google Scholar
Obleser, Jonas, Lahiri, Aditi & Eulitz, Carsten (2004). Magnetic brain response mirrors extraction of phonological features from spoken vowels. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16. 3139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohala, John J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In MacNeilage, Peter F. (ed.) The production of speech. New York: Springer. 189216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinheiro, Jose, Bates, Douglas, DebRoy, Saikat, Sarkar, Deepayan & the R Core Team. (2009). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-93.Google Scholar
Poeppel, David, Idsardi, William J. & van Wassenhove, Virginie (2008). Speech perception at the interface of neurobiology and linguistics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 363. 10711086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Development Core Team (2005). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at http://www.r-project.org.Google Scholar
Rooy, Bertus van & Wissing, Daan (2001). Distinctive [voice] implies regressive voicing assimilation. In Hall (2001). 295334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Caroline L. (1997). The devoicing of /z/ in American English: effects of local and prosodic context. JPh 25. 471500.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (1987). Redundant values. CLS 23:2. 339362.Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca (1995). Underspecification and markedness. In Goldsmith, John A. (ed.) The handbook of phonological theory. Cambridge, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell. 114174.Google Scholar
Stevens, Kenneth N. & Halle, Morris (1967). Remarks on analysis by synthesis and distinctive features. In Wathen-Dunn, Weiant (ed.) Models for the perception of speech and visual form. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. 88–102.Google Scholar