Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T14:14:09.168Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grammaticality judgments in autism: Deviance or delay*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2009

INGE-MARIE EIGSTI*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut
LOISA BENNETTO
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester
*
Address for correspondence: Inge-Marie Eigsti, Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, Unit 1020, Storrs, CT 06269. tel: (860) 486-6021; fax: (860) 486-2760; e-mail: inge-marie.eigsti@uconn.edu

Abstract

Language in autism has been the subject of intense interest, because communication deficits are central to the disorder, and because autism serves as an arena for testing theories of language acquisition. High-functioning older children with autism are often considered to have intact grammatical abilities, despite pragmatic impairments. Given the heterogeneity in language skills at younger ages, this assumption merits further investigation. Participants with autism (n=21, aged nine to seventeen years), matched on chronological age, receptive vocabulary and IQ, to 22 typically developing individuals, completed a grammaticality judgment task. Participants with autism were significantly less sensitive than controls, specifically for third person singular and present progressive marking. Performance interacted with sentence length, with lower sensitivity to errors occurring at the end of the longest stimulus sentences. Performance sensitivity was associated with onset of single word and phrase speech, and with severity of autistic symptomatology. Implications of findings are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2009 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We thank several anonymous reviewers for helpful comments, Elissa Newport for sharing her stimuli and the children and families who participated.

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders IV-TR, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S. J. & Streiner, D. (1980). Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 10(1), 3950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F. & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development 67, 1816–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A. & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry 175, 444–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, A. & Bates, E. (1995). Inducing agrammatic profiles in normals: Evidence for the selective vulnerability of morphology under cognitive resource limitations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7, 228–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botting, N. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties, and specific language impairment: Can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 45, 515–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Drew, A. & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 38(3), 265–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C. & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language 30(1), 213–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Condouris, K., Meyer, E. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech and Language Pathology 12(3), 349–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-3. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I. M., Bennetto, L. & Dadlani, M. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37(6), 10071023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eigsti, I. M. & Shapiro, T. (2003). A systems neuroscience approach to autism: Biological, cognitive and clinical perspectives. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 9, 205215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenmajer, R., Prior, M., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Ong, B., Gould, J. & Welham, M. (1998). Delayed language as a predictor of clinical symptoms in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 28, 527–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, W. A., Thorsen, K. L., Osann, K. & Spence, M. A. (2008). Use of home videotapes to confirm parental reports of regression in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38(6), 1136–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1978). A study in human capacities. Science 200(4342), 649–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermelin, B. & O'Connor, N. (1970). Psychological experiments with autistic children. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Department of Sociology.Google Scholar
Howlin, P. (1984). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: A critique and replication of the findings of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 14(2), 127–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J. & Russell, J. (1997). Language profiles in children with autism: Theoretical and methodological implications. Autism 1, 5776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S. & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology 21, 6099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P. & Udwin, O. (1997). Language and Williams syndrome: How intact is ‘intact’? Child Development 68(2), 246–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjelgaard, M. M. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes 16(2/3), 287308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F. & Saffran, E. M. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition 13, 361–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C. & Risi, S. (2002). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38(8), 1426–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mawhood, L., Howlin, P. & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder – A comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 41(5), 547–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, S., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Smith, I. & Bryson, S. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 27(2), S6978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1979). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds) Talking to children, 109149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Prior, M. R. & Hall, L. C. (1979). Comprehension of transitive and intransitive phrases by autistic, retarded, and normal children. Journal of Communication Disorders 12, 103–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapin, I. & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain and Development 25(3), 166–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 45, 360–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K. & Redmond, S. M. (1999). Grammaticality judgements of an extended optional infinitive grammar: Evidence from English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 42(4), 943–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, J. A., Rice, M. L. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics 25, 429–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Mirrett, P. & Burchinal, M. (2001). Receptive and expressive communication development of young males with Fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation 106(3), 216–30.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A. & Lord, C. (2003). ADI-R: The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H., Rescorla, L., Tager-Flusberg, H., Fowler, A. & Sudhalter, V. (1991). Relation of utterance length to grammatical complexity in normal or language-disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 2345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sivberg, B. (2003). Parents' detection of early signs in their children having an autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Pediatric Nursing 18, 433–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Boyle, M., Streiner, D. & Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of outcome among high functioning children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44(4), 520–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). Sentence comprehension in autistic children. Applied Psycholinguistics 2, 524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). A psycholinguistic perspective on language development in the autistic child. In Dawson, G. (ed.) Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment, 92115. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M. & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edn. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edn. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wilson, S. & Saygin, A. (2004). Grammaticality judgment in aphasia: Deficits are not specific to syntactic structures, aphasic syndromes, or lesion sites. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16(2), 238–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wulfeck, B. (1993). A reaction time study of grammaticality judgments in children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 1208–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed