Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T04:02:49.167Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 February 2009

HUEI-MEI LIU*
Affiliation:
Department of Special Education, National Taiwan Normal University
FENG-MING TSAO
Affiliation:
Department of PsychologyNational Taiwan University
PATRICIA K. KUHL
Affiliation:
Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences, University of Washington, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Huei-Mei Liu, Department of Special Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, TAIWAN. e-mail: liumei@ntnu.edu.tw

Abstract

Acoustic-phonetic exaggeration of infant-directed speech (IDS) is well documented, but few studies address whether these features are modified with a child's age. Mandarin-speaking mothers were recorded while addressing an adult and their child at two ages (0 ; 7–1 ; 0 and 5 ; 0) to examine the acoustic-phonetic differences between IDS and child-directed speech (CDS). CDS exhibits an exaggeration pattern resembling that of IDS – expanded vowel space, longer vowels, higher pitch and greater lexical tone differences – when compared to ADS. Longitudinal analysis demonstrated that the extent of acoustic exaggeration is significantly smaller in CDS than in IDS. Age-related changes in maternal speech provide some support for the hypothesis that mothers adjust their speech directed toward children as a function of the child's language ability.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2009 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by a research grant from the National Science Council, Taiwan, to Huei-Mei Liu (NSC 92-2413-H-003-072 & NSC 93-2413-H-003-019). Patricia K. Kuhl's contribution was supported by the Hsin-Yi Foundation of Taiwan and an NSF Science of Learning Center grant (0354453).

References

REFERENCES

Amano, S., Nakatani, T. & Kondo, T. (2006). Fundamental frequency of infants' and parents' utterances in longitudinal recordings. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119, 1636–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradlow, A. R., Torretta, G. M. & Pisoni, D. B. (1996). Intelligibility of normal speech I: Global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech Communication 20(3–4), 255–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brousseau, L., Malcuit, G., Pomerleau, A. & Feider, H. (1996). Relations between lexical-temporal features in mothers' speech and infants' interactive behaviours. First Language 16, 4159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, D., Kitamura, C. & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2002). What's new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science 296, 1435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S. & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants' preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 20(4), 477–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustment: the contributions of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds) Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 151–88. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1991). Prosody in speech to children: Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Annals of Child Development 8, 4380.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers' speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20(1), 104113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J. T. & Harshman, R. A. (1978). Crosslanguage differences in tone perception: A multidimensional scaling investigation. Language and Speech 21, 133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnica, O. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds) Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 6388. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grieser, D. L. & Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic feature in motherese. Developmental Psychology 24(1), 1420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (Modern Standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language 27, 342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanaugh, R. D. & Jirkovsky, A. M. (1982). Parental speech to young children: A longitudinal analysis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 28(2), 297311.Google Scholar
Kitamura, C. & Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: Adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy 4(1), 85110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D. & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2002). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development 24, 372–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U. & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277, 684–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M. & Nelson, T. (2008). Early phonetic perception as a gateway to language: New data and Native Language Magnet Theory, expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 363, 9791000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T. & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: Implications for the ‘Critical Period’. Language Learning and Development 1(3–4), 237–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H. M., Kuhl, P. K. & Tsao, F. M. (2003). The association between mothers' speech clarity and infants' speech discrimination skill. Developmental Science 6(3), F1F10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H. M., Tsao, F. M. & Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Acoustic analysis of lexical tone in Mandarin infant-directed speech. Developmental Psychology 43, 912–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. D., Johnson, J. & Peters, J. (1990). Fine-tuning of utterance length to preverbal infants: Effects on later language development. Journal of Child Language 17, 511–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds) Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, 109–50. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Picheny, M. A., Durlach, N. I. & Braida, L. D. (1989). Speaking clearly for the hard of hearing III: An attempt to determine the contribution of speaking rate to difference in intelligibility between clear and conversational speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 32, 600603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1994). Beginning from baby talk: Twenty years of research on input in interaction. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds) Input and interaction in language acquisition, 312. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., Barnett, R. K. & Mackain, K. (1983). The prosody of maternal speech: Infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language 10, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M. & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development 75, 1067–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, G. S., Tjaden, K. & Weismer, G. (1995). The influence of speaking rate on vowel space and speech intelligibility for individuals with Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 10011013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren-Leubecker, A. & Bohannon, J. N. (1984). Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: Mother–father differences. Child Development 55, 1379–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar