Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ph5wq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-19T00:49:24.227Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of bilingualism, noise, and reverberation on speech perception by listeners with normal hearing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2006

CATHERINE L. ROGERS
Affiliation:
University of South Florida
JENNIFER J. LISTER
Affiliation:
University of South Florida
DASHIELLE M. FEBO
Affiliation:
University of South Florida
JOAN M. BESING
Affiliation:
Montclair State University
HARVEY B. ABRAMS
Affiliation:
Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Audiology and Speech Pathology Services

Abstract

This study compared monosyllabic word recognition in quiet, noise, and noise with reverberation for 15 monolingual American English speakers and 12 Spanish–English bilinguals who had learned English prior to 6 years of age and spoke English without a noticeable foreign accent. Significantly poorer word recognition scores were obtained for the bilingual listeners than for the monolingual listeners under conditions of noise and noise with reverberation, but not in quiet. Although bilinguals with little or no foreign accent in their second language are often assumed by their peers, or their clinicians in the case of hearing loss, to be identical in perceptual abilities to monolinguals, the present data suggest that they may have greater difficulty in recognizing words in noisy or reverberant listening environments.

Type
Articles
Copyright
2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abel S. M., Alberti P. W., & Riko K.1980. Speech intelligibility in noise with ear protectors. Journal of Otolaryngology, 9, 256265.Google Scholar
Besing J., & Koehnke J.1995. A test of virtual auditory localization. Ear and Hearing, 16, 220229.Google Scholar
Besing J., Koehnke J., Fedor A., Lister J., & Febo D.2001. Evaluating listeners with normal and impaired hearing on clinical tests of spatial localization and speech intelligibility gain. Association for Research in Otolaryngology Abstracts, 395, 111.Google Scholar
Bialystock E.1988. Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 560567.Google Scholar
Bialystock E.1997. Effects of bilingualism and biliteracy on children's emerging concepts of print. Developmental Psychology, 33, 429440.Google Scholar
Bialystock E.2002. Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 52, 159199.Google Scholar
Bialystock E., Craik F. I. M., Klein R., & Viswanathan M.2004. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303.Google Scholar
Bialystock E., & Martin M. M.2004. Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325339.Google Scholar
Bialystock E., Shenfield T., & Codd J.2000. Languages, scripts, and the environment: Factors in developing concepts of print. Developmental Psychology, 36, 6676.Google Scholar
Crandell C., & Smaldino J.1996. Speech perception in noise by children for whom English is a second language. American Journal of Audiology, 5, 4751.Google Scholar
Crandell C., & Smaldino J.2000. Classroom acoustics for children with normal hearing and with hearing impairment. Journal of Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 362370.Google Scholar
Dethloff C., Besing J., & Koehnke J.1998. Effects of presentation method on virtual speech intelligibility in noise. Paper presented at the Meeting of the American Speech–Language and Hearing Association, San Antonio, TX.
Egan J. P.1948. Articulation testing methods. Laryngoscope, 58, 955991.Google Scholar
Flege J. E.1987. The production of “new” and “similar” phones in a foreign language: Evidence for the effect of equivalence classification. Journal of Phonetics, 15, 4765.Google Scholar
Flege J. E.1995. Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-linguistic research (pp. 229273). Timonium, MD: York Press.
Grosjean F.1997. Processing mixed language: Issues, findings, and models. In A. M. B. de Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 225253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Helfer K., & Huntley R.1991. Aging and consonant errors in reverberation and noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90, 17861796.Google Scholar
Helfer K., & Wilber L.1990. Hearing loss, aging, and speech perception in reverberation and in noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 149155.Google Scholar
Hirsch I., Davis H., Silverman S., Reynolds E., Eldert E., & Benson R.1952. Development of materials for speech audiometry. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 17, 321337.Google Scholar
IEEE. 1969. IEEE recommended practice for speech quality measurements. IEEE Transactions on Audio and Electroacoustics, AU-17, 225246.
Kessler C., & Quinn M. E.1980. Positive effects of bilingualism on science problem-solving abilities. In J. E. Alatis (Ed.), Current issues in bilingual education: Proceedings of the Georgetown Roundtable on Languages and Linguistics (pp. 295308). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Kessler C., & Quinn M. E.1987. Language minority children's linguistic and cognitive creativity. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 8, 173186.Google Scholar
Koehnke J., & Besing J.1996. A procedure for testing speech intelligibility in a virtual listening environment. Ear and Hearing, 17, 211217.Google Scholar
Kormi-Nouri R., Moniri S., & Nilsson L.-G.2003. Episodic and semantic memory in bilingual and monolingual children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 4754.Google Scholar
Luce P. A., Feustel T. C., & Pisoni D. B.1983. Capacity demands in short-term memory for synthetic and natural word lists. Human Factors, 25, 1732.Google Scholar
MacKay I. R. A., & Flege J. E.2004. Effects of the age of second language learning on the duration of first and second language sentences: The role of suppression. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 373396.Google Scholar
Maxwell D., & Satake E.1997. Research and statistical methods in communication disorders. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
Mayo L., Florentine M., & Buus S.1997. Age of second-language acquisition and perception of speech in noise. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 686693.Google Scholar
Meador D., Flege J. E., & Mackay I. R. A.2000. Factors affecting the recognition of words in a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 5567.Google Scholar
Miller G., Heise G., & Lichten W.1951. The intelligibility of speech as a function of the context of the speech materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 329335.Google Scholar
Moncur J., & Dirks D.1967. Binaural and monaural speech intelligibility in reverberation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 10, 186195.Google Scholar
Nabelek A.1988. Identification of vowels in quiet, noise, and reverberation: Relationships with age and hearing loss. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84, 476484.Google Scholar
Nabelek A., & Donahue A.1984. Perception of consonants in reverberation by native and non-native listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 75, 632634.Google Scholar
Nabelek A., & Mason D.1981. Effect of noise and reverberation on binaural and monaural word identification by subjects with various audiograms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 375383.Google Scholar
Nabelek A., & Pickett J.1974. Monaural and binaural speech perception through hearing aids under noise and reverberation with normal and hearing-impaired listeners. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 17, 724739.Google Scholar
Nabelek A., & Robinson P.1982. Monaural and binaural speech perception in reverberation for listeners of various ages. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71, 12421248.Google Scholar
Newman A., & Hochberg I.1983. Children's perception of speech in reverberation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 21452148.Google Scholar
Picard M., & Bradley J.2001. Revisiting speech interference in classrooms. Audiology, 40, 221244.Google Scholar
Pichora-Fuller M. K., Schneider B. A., and Daneman M.1995. How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 593608.Google Scholar
Ransdell S., Arecco R., & Levy C. M.2001. Bilingual long-term working memory: The effects of working memory loads on writing quality and fluency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 113128.Google Scholar
Rickard Liow S.1999. Reading skill development in bilingual Singaporean children. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 196213). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Steinberg J.1929. Effects of distortion on the recognition of speech sounds. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1, 1211137.Google Scholar
Southwood M. H., & Flege J. E.1999. Scaling foreign accent: Direct magnitude estimation versus interval scaling. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 13, 335349.Google Scholar
Takata Y., & Nabelek A.1990. English consonant recognition in noise and in reverberation by Japanese and American listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, 663666.Google Scholar
Therrien M., & Ramirez R.2000. The Hispanic population in the United States: March 2000, Current Population Reports, P20-535. Washington, DC: US Census Bureau.
Von Hapsburg D., & Peña E.2002. Understanding bilingualism and its impact on speech audiometry. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 202213.Google Scholar
US Bureau of the Census. 1990. Census of Population, CPHL-133. Washington, DC: US Department of Commerce.
US Bureau of the Census. 2000. Statistical abstract of the United States. Washington, DC: US Department of Commerce.
US Bureau of the Census. 2001. Supplementary survey profile. Washington, DC: US Department of Commerce.