Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:05:18.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Narrative performance and parental scaffolding of shy and nonshy children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2009

KAILEY PEARL REYNOLDS*
Affiliation:
University of Guelph
MARY ANN EVANS
Affiliation:
University of Guelph
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Kailey Pearl Reynolds, Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, CanadaN1G 2W1. E-mail: kailey@uoguelph.ca

Abstract

This study examined differences in performance between 20 shy and 20 matched nonshy children on a narrative task and in the way parents scaffolded their narrative performance when reading the wordless book Frog, Where Are You, by Mercer Mayer. Consistent with previous research, results demonstrated that shy children spoke less than their nonshy peers and volunteered less story content. Parents, however, did not differ in how they scaffolded their children's speech turns, nor in the amount of semantic information they provided. Thus, these communicative differences were not accounted for by differential adult scaffolding. Implications for encouraging more verbal behavior from shy children and for the design of wordless storybooks are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aksu-Koc, A., & Tekdemir, G. (2004). Relating events in narrative. In Strömqvist, S. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B., & Meier, G. H. (1993). Personality effects on children's speech in everyday life: Sociability-mediated exposure and shyness-mediated reactivity to social situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 10721083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Botvin, G. J., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1977). The development of structural complexity in children's fantasy narratives. Developmental Psychology, 13, 377388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, A. C., & Wang, M. (1999). Frog, Where Are You? Children's narrative expression over the telephone. Discourse Processes, 28, 217236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caprara, G. V., Steca, P., Cervone, D., & Artistico, D. (2003). The contribution of self-efficacy beliefs to dispositional shyness: On social-cognitive systems and the development of personality dispositions. Journal of Personality, 71, 943970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Beck, R. J. (1999). Maternal scaffolding and children's narrative retelling of a movie story. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 14, 409434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, T. G., Nienhuys, T. G., & Kirkman, M. (1985). Parent–child interaction with receptively disabled children: Some determinants of maternal speech style. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 5). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crozier, W. R. (1995). Shyness and self-esteem in middle childhood. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 8595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dillon, J. T. (1990). The practice of questioning. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Evans, M. A. (1987). Discourse characteristic of reticent children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 171184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. A. (1993). Communicative competence as a dimension of shyness. In Rubin, K. & Asendorpf, J. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Evans, M. A. (1996). Reticent primary grade children and their more talkative peers: Verbal, nonverbal, and self-concept characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 739749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. A. (in press). Language performance, academic performance and shy sympomatology: A comprehensive review. In Rubin, K. H. & Coplna, R. (Eds.), The development of shyness and social withdrawal. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Evans, M. A., & Bienert, H. (1992). Control and paradox in teacher conversations with shy children. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 24, 502516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. A., & Schmidt, F. (1991). Repeated maternal book reading with two children: Language normal and language impaired. First Language, 11, 269287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gewirtz, J. L. (1948). Studies in word-fluency II: Its relation to eleven items of child behaviour. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 72, 177184.Google Scholar
Hall, W. S., Nagy, W. E., & Linn, R. (1984). Spoken words: Effects of situation and social group on oral word usage and frequency. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kaderavek, J. N., & Sulzby, E. (2000). Narrative production by children with and without specific language impairment: Oral narratives and emergent reading. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 3449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (1999). The concept of behavioral inhibition. In Schmidt, L. A. & Schulkin, J. (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and social phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and clinical outcomes (pp. 313). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kemple, K. M., David, G. M. & Wang, Y. (1996). Preschoolers' creativity, shyness, and self-esteem. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 317326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual Arts. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Levin, I. P., & Hart, S. S. (2003). Risk preferences in young children: Early evidence of individual differences in reaction to potential gains and losses. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 16, 397413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analysing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. (1974). Changes in verbal child–mother interactions with increasing language skills of the child. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 3, 101116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language Communication Disorders, 38, 287313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, R., Hernandez, R., Taylor, L. & Johnson, K. (1996). Narrative development in late talkers: Early school age. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 12951303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, C. (1994). Narrative skills and social class. Canadian Journal of Education, 19, 251269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1992). Parental styles of narrative elicitation: Effect on children's narrative structure and content. First Language, 12, 299321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1986). Genetics and shyness. In Jones, W. H., Cheek, J. M., & Briggs, S. R. (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 6380). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hersey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spere, K. A., Schmidt, L. A., Theall-Honey, L. S., & Martin-Chang, S. (2004). Expressive and receptive language skills of temperamentally shy preschoolers. Infant and Child Development, 13, 123133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spooner, A. L. (2006). Hidden shyness: Child, parent, and measurement variables that allow children's shyness to go undetected (Doctoral dissertation, University of Guelph, 2006). Dissertation Abstracts International, 67, 579.Google Scholar
Strömqvist, S., & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.) (2004). Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toda, S., Fogel, A., & Kawai, M. (1990). Maternal speech to three-month-old infants in the United States and Japan. Journal of Child Language, 17, 279294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torr, J. (2004). Talking about picture books: The influence of maternal education on four-year-old children's talk with mothers and pre-school teachers. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 4, 181210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Umiker-Sebeok, D. J. (1979). Preschool children's intraconversational narratives. Journal of Child Language, 6, 90109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Kleeck, A., & Street, R. (1982). Does reticence mean just talking less? Qualitative differences in the language of talkative and reticent preschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 609629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, H., & Wood, D. (1983). Questioning the preschool child. Educational Review, 35, 149162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, H., & Wood, D. (1984). An experimental evaluation of the effects of five styles of teacher conversation on the language of hearing impaired children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 4562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimbardo, P. (1977). Shyness: What it is, what to do about it. New York: Symphony Press.Google Scholar