Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T19:22:14.664Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fillers as signs of distributional learning*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 September 2008

HELENA TAELMAN*
Affiliation:
CNTS, University of Antwerp
GERT DURIEUX
Affiliation:
CNTS, University of Antwerp
STEVEN GILLIS
Affiliation:
CNTS, University of Antwerp
*
Address for correspondence: Helena Taelman, University of Antwerp, Dept. of Linguistics – CNTS, Prinsstraat 13, B-2000 Antwerpen, Belgium. tel: 0032-3 820.27.89; e-mail: Helena.Taelman@telenet.be

Abstract

A longitudinal analysis is presented of the fillers of a Dutch-speaking child between 1 ; 10 and 2 ; 7. Our analysis corroborates familiar regularities reported in the literature: most fillers resemble articles in shape and distribution, and are affected by rhythmic and positional constraints. A novel finding is the impact of the lexical environment: particular function words act as ‘anchor’ words that attract occurrences of schwa fillers after them. The child inserts significantly more schwa fillers in these contexts. The anchor words are among the most frequent words preceding articles in the input, indicating a sharp sensitivity to such distributional regularities. Nasal fillers too are affected by distributional learning, but at the phonological level: the child first uses nasals before [h]-initial nouns, and then generalizes this usage to all [h]-initial words. These observations are related to the growing body of evidence for the impact of distributional learning on early language production.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported in this paper was enabled by a postdoctoral research grant of the National Science Foundation – FWO awarded to the first author, and a grant from the Research Council of the University of Antwerp.

References

REFERENCES

Abu-Akel, A., Bailey, A. L. & Thum, Y. M. (2004). Describing the acquisition of determiners in English: A growth modeling approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 33, 407–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassano, D. & Eme, P. E. (2001). Development of noun determiner use in French children: Lexical and grammatical bases. In Almgren, M., Barreña, A., Ezeizabarrena, M. J., Idiazabal, I. & MacWhinney, B. (eds), Research on child language acquisition. Proceedings of the 8th conference of the IASCL, 1207–20. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Bol, G. W. & Kuiken, F. (1990). Grammatical analysis of developmental language disorders: A study of the morphosyntax of children with specific language disorders, with hearing impairment and with Down's syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 4, 7786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science 27, 843–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabrowska, E. (2001). Discriminating between constructivist and nativist positions: Fillers as evidence of generalization. Journal of Child Language 28, 243–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, A. & Menn, L. (2003). Up close and personal: A case study of the development of three English fillers. Journal of Child Language 30, 735–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fikkert, P. (1994). On the acquisition of prosodic structure. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.Google Scholar
Gerken, L. A. (1996). Prosodic structure in young children's language production. Language 72, 683712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, R. L. & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition 70, 109–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haeseryn, W., Romijn, K., Geerts, G., de Rooij, J. & van den Toorn, M. C. (1997). Algemene Nederlandse spraakkunst. Groningen: Wolters-Noordhoff.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. E. (2000). What you see is what you get: The importance of transcription for interpreting children's morphosyntactic development. In Menn, L. & Bernstein Ratner, N. (eds), Methods for studying language production, 181204. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kilani-Schoch, M. & Dressler, W. U. (2001). Filler+infinitive and pre- and protomorphology demarcation in a French acquisition corpus. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 30, 653–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A. & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition 83, B35B42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, C. C. (1994). On the acquisition of place. Dordrecht: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics.Google Scholar
Lleo, C. (2001 a). Early fillers: Undoubtedly more than phonological stuffing. Journal of Child Language 28, 262–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lleo, C. (2001 b). The transition from prenominal fillers to articles in Spanish and German. In Almgren, M., Barreña, A., Ezeizabarrena, M. J., Idiazabal, I. & MacWhinney, B. (eds), Research on child language acquisition. Proceedings of the 8th conference of the IASCL, 713–37. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lopez-Ornat, S. (2001). Fillers: How much do they generalize? Journal of Child Language 28, 243–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pepinsky, T., Demuth, K. & Roark, B. (2001). The status of filler syllables in children's early speech. BUCLD 25, 575–86.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1997). Language typology, prosody, and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. In Slobin, D. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, 136–97. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (2001 a). Filler syllables: What is their status in emerging grammar? Journal of Child Language 28, 229–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. M. (2001 b). From prosody to grammar in English. The differentiation of catenatives, modals, and auxiliaries from a single protomorpheme. In Weissenborn, J. & Höhle, B. (eds), Approaches to bootstrapping. Phonological, lexical, syntactic and neurophysiological aspects of early language acquisition, 121–56. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. & Menn, L. (1993). False starts and filler syllables: Ways to learn grammatical morphemes. Language 69, 742–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M. & Lieven, E. V. M. (1997). Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category. Applied Psycholinguistics 18, 123–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M. & Martindale, H. (1996). Syntactic categories in the speech of young children: The case of the determiner. Journal of Child Language 23, 369–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L. & Aslin, R. N. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274, 1926–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Rowland, C. F. (2002). Going, going, gone: The acquisition of the verb ‘go’. Journal of Child Language 29, 783811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74, 209–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valian, V. (1986). Syntactic categories in the speech of young children. Developmental Psychology 22, 562–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Kampen, N. J. (1994). The learnability of the left branch condition. In Bok-Bennema, R. & Cremers, C. (eds), Linguistics in the Netherlands 199, 8394. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Sinclair, H. (2000). The changing status of ‘filler syllables’ on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 27, 461500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M. M. (1996). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wijnen, F. (1988). Spontaneous word fragmentations in children: Evidence for the syllable as a unit in speech production. Journal of Phonetics 16, 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wijnen, F. & Bol, G. (1993). The escape from the optional infinitive stage. In de Boer, A., de Jong, J. & Landeweerd, R. (eds), Language and cognition 3, 239–48. Groningen: University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Wijnen, F., Krikhaar, E. & den Os, E. (1994). The (non)realization of unstressed elements in children's utterances: Evidence for a rhythmic constraint. Journal of Child Language 21, 5983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed