Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T08:42:33.070Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal control strategies, maternal language usage and children's language usage at two years*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 October 2008

NICOLE TAYLOR
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin – Madison
WILBERTA DONOVAN*
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin – Madison
SALLY MILES
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin – Madison
LEWIS LEAVITT
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin – Madison
*
Address for correspondence: Wilberta Donovan, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1500 Highland Ave., Madison, WI 53705. Email: Donovan@waisman.wisc.edu.

Abstract

The present study determined whether parenting style, defined by control strategies varying in power-assertion mediated the established relation between maternal language usage (grammar and semantics) and child language (grammar, semantics and pragmatics) during toddlerhood (n=60). Based upon their use of control strategies mothers were categorized into continuum-of-control groups (i.e. high guidance (HG), high control (HC) or high negative control (HNC)). Mothers in the high negative control group, who characteristically used high levels of prohibitions and commands, had children who performed relatively poorly overall on the language measures (i.e. MLU, number of bound morphemes, number of different words and use of language functions). In contrast, children of mothers in the HG and HC groups exhibited more advanced language usage overall. The relation between maternal and child language usage was mediated by parenting style for child pragmatics and partially for child grammar.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by grants HD38378 and P30 HD03352 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Portions of these data were presented at the Symposium on Research in Child language Disorders, Madison, WI, June, 2006. We are indebted to SALT Lab for transcription of the language samples and Robin Chapman for a critical reading of the manuscript. We wish to thank Jennifer Jindrich, Emily Harms, Laura Stuntebeck and Jennifer Broder for their assistance in the data collection and coding. Lastly we wish to thank the mothers who participated in the study for their time and interest.

References

REFERENCES

Anthony, B. & Putman, L. (1985). Cardiac blink reflex concomitants of attentional selectivity: A comparison of adults and young children. Psychophysiology 22, 508–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D. & Wells, G. (1983). Characteristics of adult speech which predict children's language development. Journal of Child Language 10, 6584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, R. M. & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51, 1173–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R. A. (1995). Narrative competence and storytelling performance: How children tell stories in different contexts. Journal of Narrative and Life History 5, 283313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to language: Acquiring the power of expression. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Painter, K. M. & Genevro, J. L. (2000). Child language with mother and with stranger at home and in the laboratory: A methodological study. Journal of Child Language 27, 407–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Painter, K. M. & Park, J. (2002). Naturalistic language sampling in typically developing children. Journal of Child Language 49, 687–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L. & Johnson, M. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development 7, 350–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockenberg, S. B. & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psychology 26, 961–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, R. M., Neal, C. & Vachio, A. (1991). Maternal teaching in the zone of proximal development: A comparison of low- and high-risk dyads. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 37, 83108.Google Scholar
Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A. & Taylor, N. L. (2005). Maternal self-efficacy and experimentally manipulated infant difficulty effects on maternal sensory sensitivity: A signal detection analysis. Developmental Psychology 41, 784–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A. & Walsh, R. O. (1997). Cogitive set and coping strategy affect mothers' sensitivity in infant cries: A signal detection approach. Child Development 68, 760–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A. & Walsh, R. O. (2000). Maternal illusory control predicts socialization strategies and toddler compliance. Developmental Psychology 36, 402–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donovan, W. L., Taylor, N. & Leavitt, L. A. (2007). Maternal sensory sensitivity and response bias in detecting change in infant facial expressions: Maternal self-efficacy and infant gender labeling. Infant Behavior and Development 30, 436–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S., Murray-Branch, J. & Miller, J. F. (1994). A prospective longitudinal study of language development in late talkers. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 37, 852–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Resznick, S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S. & Reilly, J. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Developmental Inventory. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.Google Scholar
Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds) (1994). Input and interaction in language acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallaway, C. & Woll, B. (1994). Interaction and childhood deafness. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds) Input and interaction in language acquisition, 197218. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function and structure in maternal speech: Their relation to the child's development of syntax. Developmental Psychology 22, 155–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M., Dote-Kwan, J. & Dolendo, J. (1999). Characteristics of maternal directiveness and responsiveness with young children with visual impairments. Child: Care, Health and Development 25, 285–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huntsinger, E. T. & Luecken, L. J. (2004). Attachment relationships and health behavior: The mediational role of self-esteem. Psychology and Health 19, 525–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, R. E. (1995). Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences, 3rd ed. Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L. & Swank, P. (1997). Predicting cognitive-language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology 33, 1040–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the story: A longitudinal study of parental styles in eliciting narratives and developing narrative skill. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (eds) Developing narrative structure, 217–53. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miles, S., Chapman, R. S. & Sindberg, H. (2006). Research Note: Sampling context affects MLU in the language of adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 49, 325–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F. & Chapman, R. S. (2003). SALT: A computer program for the systematic analysis of language transcripts. Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. (1980). Relationships between parental input frequencies and children's language acquisition: A reanalysis of Brown's data. Journal of Child Language 7, 105–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. D. & Hornbaker, A. V. (1997). Maternal directive and facilitative interaction styles: Associations with language and cognitive development of low risk and high risk toddlers. Development and Psychopathology 9, 507–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ninio, A. & Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Pan, B. A., Perlmann, R. Y. & Snow, C. E. (2000). Food for thought: Dinner table as a context for observing parent-child discourse. In Menn, L. & Ratner, N. B. (eds) Methods for studying language production, 205–24. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M. & Murdoch, K. (1998). How what we tell becomes what we know: Listener effects of speakers' long-term memory for events. Discourse Processes 26, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. & McCabe, A. (1996). Parental scaffolding of context in children's narratives. In Gilbert, J. H. V. & Johnson, C. E. (eds) Children's language, Vol. 9, 183–96. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pine, J. M. (1994). The language of primary caregivers. In Gallaway, C. & Richards, B. J. (eds) Input and interaction in language acquisition, 1537. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressman, L., Pipp-Siegel, S., Yoshinaga-Itano, C. & Deas, A. (1999). The relation of sensitivity to child expressive language gain in deaf and hard-of-hearing children whose caregivers are hearing. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 4, 294304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rocissano, L., Slade, A. & Lynch, V. (1987). Dyadic synchrony and toddler compliance. Developmental Psychology 23, 698704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H., Wyckoff, J. & Davidson, R. (1986). A reconsideration of the relation between age and mean utterance length. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 29, 394–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary cause. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, P. (eds) The handbook of child language, 180–93. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H. & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development 72, 748–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1454–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., Notaro, P. C. & Herrera, C. (2002). Parents' emotional availability and infant emotional competence: Predictors in parent-infant attachment and emerging self-regulation. Journal of Family Psychology 16, 447–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoder, P. J., Davies, B., Bishop, K. & Munson, L. (1994). The effect of adult continuing wh-questions on conversational participation in children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 193204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed