Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T23:25:50.564Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

English focus inversion1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2008

PETER W. CULICOVER*
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
SUSANNE WINKLER*
Affiliation:
University of Tübingen
*
Authors' addresses: (Culicover) Department of Linguistics, The Ohio State University, 222 Oxley Hall, 1712 Neil Ave., Columbus, OH 43210-1298, U.S.A.culicover.1@osu.edu
(Winkler) Englisches Seminar, Universität Tübingen, Wilhelmstr. 50, D-72074 Tübingen, Germany. Susanne.Winkler@T-online.de

Abstract

Besides the canonical Subject–I–VP structure, English has several inversion constructions in which the subject follows the inflected verb. The most familiar is Subject Auxiliary Inversion (SAI) which is analyzed as an instance of Head Movement (I–to–C-movement across the subject) in the generative tradition. In this paper we investigate Comparative Inversion (CI), which appears to be a special case of SAI in which ellipsis is required (Merchant 2003). Contrary to this analysis, we show that the subject can stay low in a noncanonical position, violating the Extended Projection Principle (EPP) in exactly those instances where it is under comparison and therefore heavily accented and contrastively focused. Our analysis shows that the non-application of the EPP is tied to regular interactions of syntax with phonology and syntax with semantics. We extend this in depth analysis to other English focus inversions and provide evidence that phonological highlighting and focus on the low subject can suspend the EPP. Thus, our analysis supports research programs which assume minimal syntactic structure and operations in interaction with interface constraints that are independently required for explanation.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[1]

The research on which this article is based was initiated while the first author was a visiting scholar at the University of Tübingen. This visit was made possible by an award from the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation. We are grateful to the Humboldt Foundation, Dean John Roberts and the College of Humanities of the Ohio State University, and hosts Erhard Hinrichs and Marga Reis for their support. Portions of this paper were presented to audiences at the University of Tübingen, the University of Göttingen and the University of Potsdam. We thank Michael Rochemont, Gisbert Fanselow, Ray Jackendoff, Valéria Molnár, Teresa Parodi, Shravan Vasishth, Alan Munn, Kyle Johnson, Ans van Kemenade, and Jason Merchant for their constructive advice, feedback and discussion. We are also grateful to two anonymous JL referees for their comments, and to Orin Gensler and Ewa Jaworska for perceptive and thoughtful editing of the manuscript. We are solely responsible for the errors that remain.

References

REFERENCES

Baltin, Mark. 2007. Deletion versus pro-forms: A false dichotomy? Ms., Department of Linguistics, New York University.Google Scholar
Birner, Betty J. 1996. The discourse function of inversion in English (Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics). New York: Garland Publishing.Google Scholar
Birner, Betty J. & Ward, Gregory. 1998. Information status and noncanonical word order in English. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Rong. 2003. English inversion: A ground-before-figure construction. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Barriers (Linguistic Inquiry Monographs 13). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program (Current Studies in Linguistics 28). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Lasnik, Howard. 1993. The theory of principles and parameters. In Jacobs, (eds.), 506569.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. 1999. Syntactic nuts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. & Jackendoff, Ray. 2005. Simpler syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. & Levine, Robert D.. 2001. A reconsideration of locative inversion. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 19, 283310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. & Rochemont, Michael S.. 1983. Stress and focus in English. Language 59, 123165.Google Scholar
Emonds, Joseph. 1970. Root and structure preserving transformations. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Féry, Caroline & Samek-Lodovici, Vieri. 2006. Focus projection and prosodic prominence in nested foci. Language 82.1, 131150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J., Kay, Paul & O'Connor Catherine, Mary. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64.3, 501539.Google Scholar
Gergel, Remus, Gengel, Kirsten & Winkler, Susanne. 2007. Ellipsis and inversion: A feature-based account. In Kerstin, Schwabe & Susanne, Winkler (eds.), On information structure, meaning and form (Linguistics Today 100), 301322. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Göbbel, Edward. 2007. Extraposition as PF movement. In Erin, Bainbridge & Brian, Agbayani (eds.), The Thirty-Fourth Western Conference on Linguistics (WECOL 2006), 132145. Fresno, CA: Department of Linguistics, California State University, Fresno.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Haddican, Bill. 2007. The structural deficiency of verbal pro-forms. Linguistic Inquiry 38, 539547.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael A. K. 1967. Notes on transitivity and theme in English, part II. Journal of Linguistics 3, 199244.Google Scholar
Hartmann, Katharina. 2000. Right node raising and gapping: Interface conditions on prosodic deletion. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hartmann, Katharina. 2003. Background matching in right node raising constructions. In Schwabe, & Winkler, (eds.), 121151.Google Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2004. Complexity and efficiency in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, Joachim, von Stechow, Arnim, Sternefeld, Wolfgang & Vennemann, Theo (eds.). 1993. Syntax: An international handbook of contemporary research, vol. 1. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Johnson, Kyle. 1991. Object positions. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 9, 577636.Google Scholar
Klein, Wolfgang. 1993. Ellipse. In Jacobs, (eds.), 763799.Google Scholar
Krifka, Manfred. 1999. Additive particles under stress. SALT 8, 111128. Ithaca, NY: Cornell CLC Publications.Google Scholar
Krifka, Manfred. 2007. Basic notions of information structure. In Féry, Caroline, Fanselow, Gisbert & Krifka, Manfred (eds.), Working Papers of the SFB632 (Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure (ISIS) 6), 1356. Potsdam: Universitätsverlag Potsdam.Google Scholar
Lasnik, Howard. 1999. Pseudogapping puzzles. In Lappin, Shalom & Benmamoun, Elabbas (eds.), Fragments: Studies in ellipsis and gapping, 141174. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, Luis & Winkler, Susanne. 2003. Variation at the syntax–semantics interface: Evidence from gapping. In Schwabe, & Winkler, (eds.), 227248.Google Scholar
Merchant, Jason. 2003. Subject-auxiliary inversion in comparatives and PF output constraints. In Schwabe, & Winkler, (eds.), 5577.Google Scholar
Molnár, Valéria. 2002. Contrast – from a contrastive perspective. In Hasselgård, Hilde, Johansson, Stig, Behrens, Bergljot & Fabricius-Hansen, Catherine (eds.), Information structure in a cross-linguistic perspective, 147162. Amsterdam & New York: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Molnár, Valéria. 2006. On different kinds of contrast. In Molnár, Valéria & Winkler, Susanne (eds.), The architecture of focus (Studies in Generative Grammar), 197233. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Niinuma, Fumikazu & Park, Myung-Kwan. 2004. A case for head movement at PF: SAI in comparatives. In Anne, Breitbarth & Henk van, Riemsdijk (eds.), Triggers, 431450. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, David & Torrego, Esther. 2000. T–to–C movement: Causes and consequences. In Michael, Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language, 355426. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rochemont, Michael S. 1978. A theory of stylistic rules in English. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Rochemont, Michael S. 1986. Focus in generative grammar. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochemont, Michael S. & Culicover, Peter W.. 1990. English focus constructions and the theory of grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rooth, Mats E. 1992. A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics 1, 75116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothstein, Susan. 1983. The syntactic forms of predication. Ph.D dissertation, MIT. [Circulated by Indiana University Linguistics Club, 1985.]Google Scholar
Sag, Ivan A. 1997. English relative clause constructions. Journal of Linguistics 33.2, 431484.Google Scholar
Samek-Lodovici, Vieri. 2004. Prosody–syntax interaction in the expression of focus. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 23, 687755.Google Scholar
Schwabe, Kerstin & Winkler, Susanne (eds.). 2003. The interfaces: Deriving and interpreting omitted structures (Linguistics Today 61). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Schwarzschild, Roger. 1999. Givenness, AvoidF and other constraints on the placement of accent. Natural Language Semantics 7, 141177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1995. Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing. In John, Goldsmith (ed.), The handbook of phonological theory, 550569. Oxford & Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 2004. Bengali intonation revisited: An optimality theoretic analysis in which FOCUS stress prominence drives FOCUS phrasing. In Lee, Chungmin, Gordon, Matthew & Büring, Daniel (eds.), Topic and focus: Cross-linguistic perspectives on intonation and meaning, 217246. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 2005. Comments on intonational phrasing in English. In Frota, Sónia, Vigário, Marina & João Freitas, Maria (eds.), Prosodies: With special reference to Iberian languages, 1158. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Tancredi, Chris. 1992. Deletion, de-accenting, and presupposition. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 1995. Phonological phrases: Their relation to syntax, focus, and prominence. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Ward, Gregory, Birner, Betty J. & Huddleston, Rodney. 2002. Information packaging. In Huddleston, Rodney & Pullum, Geoffrey K., The Cambridge grammar of the English language, 13631447. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wasow, Thomas. 2002. Postverbal behavior. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Williams, Edwin S. 1980. Predication. Linguistic Inquiry 11, 203238.Google Scholar
Winkler, Susanne. 2005. Ellipsis and focus in generative grammar (Studies in Generative Grammar 81). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar