Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-5xszh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T23:59:46.328Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Anat Ninio, Language and the learning curve: A new theory of syntactic development. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2006. Pp. 224. ISBN: 978-0-19-929982-9, 978-0-19-929981-2.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 October 2008

Lorraine McCune
Affiliation:
Rutgers University

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Barabasi, A.-L. & Albert, R. (1999). Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science 286, 509512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barabasi, A.-L. & Bonabeau, E. (2003). Scale-free networks. Scientific American 288, 60–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition 11, 211–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. In Bower, G. H. (ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, 164. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Herr-Israel, E. (2006). Single words to combinations: Longitudinal analysis reveals constructive processes integrating lexical and pragmatic development within a conversational context. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClure, K. & Pine, J. M. (2002). Examining the Verb Island hypothesis. Paper presented at the Joint meeting of the 9th International Congress of the International Association of the Study of Child Language and the 23rd Annual Symposium on Research in Child language Disorders (IASCL/SRCLD), Madison, Wisconsin.Google Scholar
McCune, L. (2008). How children learn to learn language. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, T. E. (1997). Describing morphosyntax: A guide for field linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. (1989). Self-organization in developmental processes: Can systems approaches work? In Gunnar, M. R. & Thelen, E. (eds) The Minnesota symposia on child psychology: volume 22. Systems and development, 77117. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theories. In Lerner, R. M. & Damon, W. (eds) Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1 Theoretical models of human development, 6th ed., 258312. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Brooks, P. J. (1998). Young children's earliest transitive and intransitive constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9, 379–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1984/1963). Symbol formation. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1978/1953). Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Zipf, G. K. (1965/1935). Psychobiology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar