Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T15:06:34.668Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bugotu and Cheke Holo reduplication: in defence of the Emergence of the Unmarked*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 June 2008

Robert Kennedy
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara

Abstract

This paper provides an analysis of the reduplicative systems of Bugotu, Cheke Holo, Tuvaluan and Hoava. In Bugotu and Cheke Holo, intervocalic consonants are not reflected in the reduplicative substring; Tuvaluan reduplication creates initial geminate consonants, while Hoava reduplicants include coda consonants. Blevins (2003, 2005) argues that these languages pose a serious problem for the optimality-theoretic hypothesis of the Emergence of the Unmarked (McCarthy & Prince 1994), which predicts that reduplicated structures should not favour relatively marked outputs. I show that the data in fact are not problematic for the Emergence of the Unmarked hypothesis, because different, conflicting dimensions of markedness can arise in reduplicative phonology. In Bugotu and Cheke Holo, the tolerance of extrasyllabic segments combined with size-penalising constraints helps produce the consonant-deletion pattern. A similar defence of the Emergence of the Unmarked is offered for the reduplicative systems of Tuvaluan and Hoava.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anttila, Arto (1997). Deriving variation from grammar. In Hinskens, Frans, van Hout, Roeland & Leo Wetzels, W. (eds.) Variation, change and phonological theory. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. 3568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anttila, Arto & Yu Cho, Young-mee (1998). Variation and change in Optimality Theory. Lingua 104. 3156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auger, Julie (2001). Phonological variation and Optimality Theory: evidence from word-initial vowel epenthesis in Vimeu Picard. Language Variation and Change 13. 253303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagemihl, Bruce (1991). Syllable structure in Bella Coola. LI 22. 589646.Google Scholar
Besnier, Niko (2000). Tuvaluan: a Polynesian language of the Central Pacific. London & New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2003). A note on reduplication in Bugotu and Cheke Holo. Oceanic Linguistics 42. 499505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, Juliette (2005). The role of phonological predictability in sound change: privileged reduction in Oceanic reduplicated substrings. Oceanic Linguistics 44. 517526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, Karen (2003). A grammar of the Hoava Language, Western Solomons. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Downing, Laura J. (1998). On the prosodic misalignment of onsetless syllables. NLLT 16. 152.Google Scholar
Gafos, Diamandis (1998). A-templatic reduplication. LI 29. 515527.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce (1982). Extrametricality and English stress. LI 13. 227276.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry (1985). A theory of phonological weight. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon (1989). Prosodic constituency in the lexicon. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Ivens, W. G. (1933). A grammar of the language of Bugotu, Ysabel Island, Solomon Islands. Bulletin of the School of Oriental Studies 7. 141177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivens, W. G. (1940). A dictionary of the language of Bugotu, Santa Isabel Island, Solomon Islands. London: Royal Asiatic Society.Google Scholar
Kager, René (1999). Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kager, René (2001). Rhythmic directionality by positional licensing. Handout of paper presented at the 5th Holland Institute of Linguistics Phonology Conference, University of Postdam. Available as ROA-514 from the Rutgers Optimality Archive.Google Scholar
Kaye, Jonathan D. & Lowenstamm, Jean (1984). De la syllabicité. In Dell, François, Hirst, Daniel & Vergnaud, Jean-Roger (eds.) Forme sonore du langage. Paris: Hermann. 123159.Google Scholar
Kennedy, Robert (2002). A stress-based approach to Ponapean reduplication. WCCFL 21. 222235.Google Scholar
Kennedy, Robert (2005). The binarity effect in Kosraean reduplication. Phonology 22. 145168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (2003). Syllables and moras in Arabic. In Féry, Caroline & Vijver, Ruben (eds.) The syllable in Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 147182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, Richard S. (1979). The phonological history of the Bugotu-Nggelic languages and its implications for Eastern Oceanic. Oceanic Linguistics 18. 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (2003). OT constraints are categorical. Phonology 20. 75138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Alan, Prince (1993a). Generalized alignment. Yearbook of Morphology 1993. 79153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Alan, Prince (1993b). Prosodic morphology I: constraint interaction and satisfaction. Ms, University of Massachusetts & Rutgers University.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Alan, Prince (1994). The emergence of the unmarked: optimality in prosodic morphology. NELS 24. 333379.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Alan, Prince (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Beckman, Jill, Dickey, Laura & Urbanczyk, Suzanne (eds.) Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst: GLSA. 249384.Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. & Alan, Prince (1999). Faithfulness and identity in Prosodic Morphology. In Kager, René, van der Hulst, Harry & Zonneveld, Wim (eds.) The prosody–morphology interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 218309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, Naomi & Reynolds, Bill (1997). Optimality Theory and variable word-final deletion in Faetar. Language Variation and Change 9. 3755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ọla, Orie, , ỌlanikỌdote; (1995). Optimality in Benue-Congo prosodic phonology and morphology. PhD dissertation, University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
Poser, William J. (1985). There is no domain size parameter. GLOW Newsletter 14. 6667.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Paul, Smolensky (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Ms, Rutgers University & University of Colorado, Boulder. Published 2004, Malden, Mass. & Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Spaelti, Philip (1997). Dimensions of variation in multi-pattern reduplication. PhD dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Ussishkin, Adam (2000). The emergence of fixed prosody. PhD dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Walker, Rachel (1998). Nasalization, neutral segments and opacity effects. PhD dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
White, Geoffrey M. (1995). Maringe (Cheke Holo). In Tryon, Darrell (ed.) Comparative Austronesian dictionary: an introduction to Austronesian studies. Part 1: Fascicle 2. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 787791.Google Scholar
White, Geoffrey M., Francis, Kokhonigita & Hugo, Pulomana (1988). Cheke Holo (Maringe/Hograno) dictionary. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Yu, Alan C.L. (2005). Quantity, stress and reduplication in Washo. Phonology 22. 437475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zoll, Cheryl (1993). Directionless syllabification and ghosts in Yawelmani. Ms, University of California, Berkeley. Available as ROA-28 from the Rutgers Optimality Archive.Google Scholar