Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ph5wq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T21:55:49.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

MORPHOSYNTAX IN THE BILINGUAL MENTAL LEXICON

An Experimental Study of Strong Stems in German

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2014

Helena Krause
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism (PRIM), University of Potsdam
Sina Bosch
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism (PRIM), University of Potsdam
Harald Clahsen*
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism (PRIM), University of Potsdam
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Harald Clahsen, Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism, University of Potsdam, 14476 Potsdam, Germany. E-mail: harald.clahsen@uni-potsdam.de

Abstract

Although morphosyntax has been identified as a major source of difficulty for adult (nonnative) language learners, most previous studies have examined a limited set of largely affix-based phenomena. Little is known about word-based morphosyntax in late bilinguals and of how morphosyntax is represented and processed in a nonnative speaker’s lexicon. To address these questions, we report results from two behavioral experiments investigating stem variants of strong verbs in German (which encode features such as tense, person, and number) in groups of advanced adult learners as well as native speakers of German. Although the late bilinguals were highly proficient in German, the results of a lexical priming experiment revealed clear native-nonnative differences. We argue that lexical representation and processing relies less on morphosyntactic information in a nonnative than in a native language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Amenta, S., & Crepaldi, D. (2012). Morphological processing as we know it: An analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 232. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbour, J. S. (1982). Productive and non-productive morphology: The case of the German strong verbs. Journal of Linguistics, 18, 331354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beedham, C. (1994). The role of consonants in marking strong verb conjugation in German and English. Folia Linguistica, 28, 279296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beedham, C. (1995/1996). Vowel+consonant and consonant+vowel sequences in the strong verbs of German and English. Cahiers Ferdinand de Saussure, 49, 139163.Google Scholar
Beretta, A., Campbell, C., Carr, T. H., Huang, J., Schmitt, L. M., Christianson, K., & Cao, Y. (2003). An ER-fMRI investigation of morphological inflection in German reveals that the brain makes a distinction between regular and irregular forms. Brain and Language, 85, 6792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bittner, A. (1996). Starke “schwache” Verben–schwache “starke” Verben. Struktur des deutschen Verbsystems [Strong “weak” verbs–weak “strong” verbs. The structure of the German verb system]. Tübingen, Germany: Stauffenburg.Google Scholar
Bittner, A., & Köpcke, K.-M. (2007). Überlegungen zur Repräsentation grammatischen Wissens am Beispiel der Verbmorphologie des Deutschen [Thoughts on the representation of grammatical knowledge: The case of German morphology]. In Di Meola, C. (Ed.), Perspektiven Zwei. Akten der 2. Tagung Deutsche Sprachwissenschaft in Italien (Rom, 9.–11.2.2006) (pp. 315). Rome, Italy: Istituto Italiano di Studi Germanici.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., & Newman, J. E. (1994). Are stem changes as natural as affixes? Linguistics, 32, 3334.Google Scholar
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject–verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 161174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Eisenbeiß, S., Hadler, M., & Sonnenstuhl, I. (2001). The mental representation of inflected words: An experimental study of adjectives and verbs in German. Language, 77, 510543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006a). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006b). How native-like is non-native language processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 564570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H., Felser, C., Sato, M., & Silva, R. (2010). Morphological structure in native and nonnative language processing. Language Learning, 60, 2143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Prüfert, P., Eisenbeiß, S., & Cholin, J. (2002). Strong stems in the German mental lexicon: Evidence from child language acquisition and adult processing. In Kaufmann, I. & Stiebels, B. (Eds.), More than words: A Festschrift for Dieter Wunderlich (pp. 91112). Berlin, Germany: Akademie-Verlag.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Sonnenstuhl, I., & Blevins, J. P. (2003). Derivational morphology in the German mental lexicon: A dual mechanism account. In Baayen, H. & Schreuder, R. (Eds.), Morphological structure in language processing (pp. 125156). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G., & Fraser, N. (1993). Network morphology: A DATR account of Russian nominal inflection. Journal of Linguistics, 29, 113142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2005). What makes learning second-language grammar difficult? A review of issues. Language Learning, 55(S1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M., & Véronique, D. (2001). Gender assignment and gender agreement in advanced French interlanguage: A cross-sectional study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 275297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimroth, C. (2008). Age effects on the process of L2 acquisition? Evidence from the acquisition of negation and finiteness in L2 German. Language Learning, 58, 117150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Plunkett, K., & Parisi, D. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fabricius-Hansen, C. (1977). Zur Klassifizierung der starken Verben im Neuhochdeutschen [Classification of strong verbs in New High German]. Deutsche Sprache, 3, 193205.Google Scholar
Feldman, L., & Fowler, C. (1987). The inflected noun system in Serbo-Croatian: Lexical representation of morphological structure. Memory & Cognition, 15, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Günther, H. (1988). Oblique word forms in visual word recognition. Linguistics, 26, 583600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale, K. & Keyser, S. J. (Eds.), The view from building 20 (pp. 111176). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haznedar, B. (2001). The acquisition of the IP system in child L2 English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heister, J., Würzner, K., & Bubenzer, J. (2011). dlexdb—eine lexikalische Datenbank für die psychologische und linguistische Forschung [dlexdb—a lexical database for psychological and linguistic research]. Psychologische Rundschau, 62, 1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T., & Wexler, K. (2002). Why is “is” easier than “-s”? Acquisition of tense/agreement morphology by child second language learners of English. Second Language Research, 18, 95136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazanina, N., Dukova-Zheleva, G., Geber, D., Kharlamov, V., & Tonciulescu, K. (2008). Decomposition into multiple morphemes during lexical access: A masked priming study of Russian nouns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 800823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, G. (2009). Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59, 503535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, K.-M. (1998). Prototypisch starke und schwache Verben der deutschen Gegenwartssprache [Prototypically strong and weak verbs in present-day German]. Germanistische Linguistik, 141/142, 4560.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (1998). Case and tense in the “fossilized” steady state. Second language Research, 14, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukatela, G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. (1987). Lexical representation of regular and irregular inflected nouns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukatela, G., Gligorijevic, B., Kostić, A., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). Representation of inflected nouns in the internal lexicon. Memory & Cognition, 8, 415423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukatela, G., Mandić, Z., Gligorijevic, B., Kostić, A., Savic, M., & Turvey, M. T. (1978). Lexical decision for inflected nouns. Language and Speech, 21, 166173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. (2007). Morphological processes in language comprehension. In Gaskell, G. (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 175193). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L., Waksler, R., & Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 101, 333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Patterson, K. (2002). Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 465472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2011). Bilingual language acquisition and theories of diachronic change: Bilingualism as cause and effect of grammatical change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 121145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R. (2005). An ERP study of second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 12121228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parodi, T. (2000). Finiteness and verb placement in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 16, 355381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16, 103133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raveh, M. (2002). The contribution of frequency and semantic similarity to morphological processing. Brain and Language, 81, 312325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, M., & Felser, C. (2010). Sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations in English as a second language. Second Language, 9, 101118.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2008). Meaning in the second language. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slioussar, N., Kireevc, M., Chernigovskayab, V., Kataevac, G., Korotkovc, A., & Medvedevc, S. (2014). An ER-fMRI study of Russian inflectional morphology. Brain and Language, 130, 3341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smolka, E., Zwitserlood, P., & Rösler, F. (2007). Stem access in regular and irregular inflection: Evidence from German participles. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 325347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanners, R. J., Neiser, J. J., Hernon, P. W., & Hall, R. (1979). Memory representation for morphologically related words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 399412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockall, L., & Marantz, A. (2006). A single route, full decomposition model of morphological complexity. The Mental Lexicon, 1, 85123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolz, J. A., & Feldman, L. B. (1995). The role of orthographic and semantic transparency of the base morpheme in morphological processing. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 109154). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 173204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & Warren, T. (2010). Beginning adult L2 learners’ sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 10921106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Jegerski, J. (2010). Research in second language processing and parsing. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veríssimo, J., & Clahsen, H. (2009). Morphological priming by itself: A study of Portuguese conjugations. Cognition, 112, 187194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and universal grammar. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wunderlich, D. (1996). Minimalist morphology: The role of paradigms. In Booji, G. & van Marle, J. (Ed.), Yearbook of morphology 1995 (pp. 93114). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar