Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T01:46:19.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grammatical gender selection and phrasal word order in child heritage Spanish: A feature re-assembly approach*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 January 2015

ALEJANDRO CUZA*
Affiliation:
Purdue University
ROCÍO PÉREZ-TATTAM
Affiliation:
Swansea University
*
Address for correspondence: Alejandro Cuza, Department of Spanish and Portuguese, School of Languages and Cultures, 640 Oval Drive. Stanley Coulter Hall, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907acuza@purdue.edu

Abstract

The present study examines the development of grammatical gender assignment, agreement, and noun-adjective word order in child heritage Spanish among thirty-two Spanish–English bilingual children born and raised in the United States. A picture-naming task revealed significant overextension of the masculine form and high levels of ungrammatical word order strings. There were no significant differences by age regarding gender concord or noun-adjective word order. We argue that the differences found can be accounted for in terms of a re-assembly of gender features leading to both morphological and syntactic variability. This approach allows for subsequent morphosyntactic shifts during early childhood depending on patterns of language use, and conceptualizes heritage language variation along the lines of current linguistic theorizing regarding the role of innate linguistic principles and language experience in language development.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank Liliana Sánchez, Ana Teresa Pérez-Leroux and Conchita Lleó for their thoughtful suggestions and feedback. We are very grateful to the three anonymous reviewers and to Ludovica Serratrice for their excellent comments and revisions. We are deeply indebted to our participants, parents, and school administrators for all of their support and collaboration. This research would not have been possible without their generous support. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 42nd Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LSRL), and we thank the audience for their questions and comments.

References

Alarcón, I. V. (2011). Spanish grammatical gender under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ linguistic behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (3), 332350.Google Scholar
Amaral, L., & Roeper, T. (2014). Multiple grammars and second language representation. Second Language Research, 30 (1), 336.Google Scholar
Austin, J., Blume, M., & Sánchez, L. (2013). Morphosyntactic Attrition in the L1 of Spanish–English Bilingual Children. Hispania, 96 (3), 542561.Google Scholar
Bernstein, J. (1993). Topics in the Syntax of nominal structures across Romance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Graduate Center. City University of New York.Google Scholar
Bosque, I., & Picallo, , , C. (1996). Postnominal adjectives in Spanish DPs. Journal of Linguistics, 32, 349385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L. (2002). The second language acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In Pérez-Leroux, A.T. & Liceras, J. (eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax: The L1/L2 connection (pp. 153178). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Carstens, V. (1991). The morphology and syntax of determiner phrases in Kiswahili. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California at Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G. (1998). References to kinds across languages. Natural Language Semantics, 6, 339405.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G., Guasti, M.T., & Gualmini, A. (1999). Nouns and articles in child grammar and the syntax/semantics map. Paper presented at GALA, Potsdam, September, 1999.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. (1994). On the evidence for partial N-movement in the Romance DP. In Cinque, G., Koster, J., Pollock, J.Y., Rizzi, L., & Zanuttini, R. (eds), Paths towards Universal Grammar: Studies in Honor of Richard S. Kayne (pp. 85110). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., & Frank, J. (2014). On the role of experience and age-related effects: Evidence from the Spanish CP. Second Language Research. Published online first on September 1. DOI: 10.1177/0267658314532939 Google Scholar
Cuza, A. (2013). Cross-linguistic influence at the syntax proper: interrogative subject-verb inversion in heritage Spanish. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 7196.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., Pérez-Tattam, R., Barajas, E., Miller, L., & Sadowski, C. (2013). The development of tense and aspect morphology in child and adult heritage Spanish: Implications for heritage language pedagogy. In Schwieter, J. (ed.), Innovative research and practices in second language acquisition and bilingualism (pp. 193220). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Demonte, V. (2008). Meaning-form correlations and adjective position in Spanish. In Kennedy, C., C. & Mc Nally, L.. (eds.) The semantics of adjectives and adverbs (pp. 71100). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demonte, V. (1999). El adjetivo: classes y usos. La posición del adjetivo en el sintagma nominal . In Bosque, I., & Demonte, V.. (eds.). Gramática Descriptiva de la Lengua Español (pp.129215). Fundación Ortega y Gasset, Madrid: Espasa Calpe.Google Scholar
Döpke, S. (1998). Competing language structures: The acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German–English children. Journal of Child Language, 25, 555584.Google Scholar
Dunn, A.L., & Fox Tree, J.E. (2009). A quick, gradient bilingual dominance scale. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12 (3), 273289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franceschina, F. (2005). Fossilized second language grammars: The acquisition of grammatical gender. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gavarró, A., Pérez-Leroux, A.T., & Roeper, T. (2006). Bare noun/DP contrasts in child Catalan. In Torrens, V. & Escobar, L. (eds.), The Romance Turn (pp. 5168). Amsterdam, John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C., Sharp, K., Pérez-Tattam, R., Stadthagen-González, H., Laporte, N., & Thomas, E.M. (in preparation). The Effect of Metalinguistic Awareness in the Acquisition of a Complex Grammatical Gender System.Google Scholar
Gollan, T.H., Weissberger, G., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R.I., & Cera, C.M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multi-lingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594615.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V.F., & Kreiter, J. (2003). Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 267288.Google Scholar
Harris, J. (1991). The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22 (1), 2762.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Franceschina, F. (2004). Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In Prévost, P. & Paradis, J. (eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts (pp. 175205). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hulk, A., & Müller, N. (2000). Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 227244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, I., Salmon, D.P., & Gollan, T.H., (2013). The Multilingual Naming Test in Alzheimer's disease: Clues to the origin of naming impairments. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19, 272283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T. (2003). Cross-Linguistic Influence in the Acquisition of Determiners in German-Italian Bilingual. In Beachley, B., Brown, A. & Conlin, F. (eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 27(2) (pp. 461472). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T. (2006). The acquisition of determiners in bilingual German-Italian and German-French children. München: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T. (2007). Determiners in bilingual German-Italian children: What they tell us about the relation between language influence and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10 (1), 5778.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2008). Feature-Assembly in Second Language Acquisition. In Liceras, J., Zobl, H., & Goodluck, H. (eds.), The role of formal features in second language acquisition (pp. 106140). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2005). On morphological competence. In Dekydtspotter, L., Sprouse, R., A. & Liljestrand, A., Proceedings of the 7th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 178192). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (1998). Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent L2 end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14 (4), 359375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J.M., Fernández Fuertes, R., Perales, S., Pérez-Tattam, R., & Spradlin, K.T. (2008). Gender and number agreement in bilingual native and non-native grammars: a view from child and adult functional-lexical mixings. Lingua, 118 (6), 827851.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (1997). Filler syllables, proto-articles and early prosodic constraints in Spanish and German. In Sorace, A., Heycock, C. & Shillcock, R. (eds), Proceedings of GALA’97 Conference on Language Acquisition (pp. 251–56). Edinburgh: The University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (1998). Proto-articles in the acquisition of Spanish: Interface between Phonology and Morphology. In Fabri, R., Ortmann, A. & Parodi, T. (eds.), Modelle der Flexion: 18. Jahrestagung der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft. Tübingen: Niemeyer Verlag.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (2001). The interface of phonology and syntax: The emergence of the article in the early acquisition of Spanish and German. In Weissenborn, J., & Höhle, B. (eds.), Approaches to Bootstrapping: Phonological, Syntactic and Neurophysiological Aspects of Early Language Acquisition (pp. 2344). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., & Demuth, K. (1999). Prosodic constraints on the emergence of grammatical morphemes: Crosslinguistic evidence from Germanic and Romance Languages. In Greenhill, A., Littlefield, H. & Tano, C. (eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 407418). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S. (1997). What lies in between a pre-grammatical and a grammatical representation? Evidence on nominal and verbal form–function mappings in Spanish from 1;7 to 2;1. In Pérez-Leroux, A. T. & Glass, W. R. (eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the acquisition of Spanish: Developing grammars (pp. 320). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S. (2008). Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: starting small. Journal of Child Language, 35, 129.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S. (1997). El proceso de gramaticalización de las categorías gramaticales en español. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
McCarthy, C. (2007). Morphological variability in second language Spanish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. McGill University, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (2007). The weaker language in early child bilingualism: Acquiring a first language as a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 495514.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism. Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58 (3), 503553.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2002). Incomplete acquisition and attrition of Spanish tense/aspect distinctions in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (1), 3968.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K. (2007). Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11 (3), 301328.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Sánchez-Walker, N. (2013). Differential Object Marking in Child and Adult Spanish Heritage Speakers. Language Acquisition, 20, 109132.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Davidson, J., de la Fuente, I., & Foote, R. (2014). Early language experience facilitates the processing of gender agreement in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (1), 118138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Marchak, K. (2011). Le carte blanc or la carte blanche? Bilingual children's acquisition of French adjective agreement. Language Learning, 61, 734758.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Assessing the effects of bilingualism: A background. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (eds.), Language and Literacy in Bilingual Children (pp. 321). Multilingual Matters. Clevedon, UK.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic Acquisition in Bilingual Children: Autonomous or independent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 6, 91124.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., & Crago, M. (2007). French-English bilingual children's acquisition of past tense. In Caunt-Nulton, H., Kulatilake, S. & Woo, I. (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 497507). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (1993). Linguistic, psycholinguistic, and neurolinguistic aspects of “interference” in bilingual speakers: the Activation Threshold Hypothesis. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 9, 133145.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1989). The acquisition of morphemes: some evidence from Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18 (3), 289312.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1991). The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18, 571590.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A.T., Cuza, A., & Thomas, D. (2011). Clitic placement in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (2), 221232.Google Scholar
Picallo, C. (1991). Nominals and nominalizations in Catalan. Probus 3, 279316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picallo, C. (2008). Gender and Number in Romance. Lingue e Linguaggio, 1, 4766.Google Scholar
Pirvulescu, M., Roberge, Y., Thomas, D., Pérez-Leroux, A.T., Strik, N. (2014). Bilingual effects: exploring object omission in pronominal languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 495510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2011). Reanalysis in adult heritage language: A case for attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33, 305–28.Google Scholar
Potowski, K., Jegerski, J., & Morgan-Short, K. (2009). The effects of instruction on linguistic development in Spanish heritage language speakers. Language Learning, 59, 537579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, M., & Sánchez, L. (2013). What's so incomplete about incomplete acquisition? A prolegomenon to modeling heritage language grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3 (4), 478508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., Judy, T., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., & Pires, A. (2010). On the (Un)—Ambiguity of Adjectival Interpretation in L2 Spanish: Informing Debates on the Mental Representation of L2 syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32 (1), 4777.Google Scholar
Roeper, T. (1999). Universal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 169186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sánchez, L. (2004). Functional Convergence in the Tense, Evidentiality and Aspectual Systems of Quechua-Spanish Bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7 (2), 147162.Google Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2014). Bilingual Language Acquisition: Spanish and English in the First Six Years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sharp, M. K. (2012). Morphosyntactic Complexity and Exposure in the Acquisition of Gender in Welsh. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Bangor University.Google Scholar
Teschner, R., & William, R. (1984). The gender patterns of Spanish nouns: An inverse dictionary-based analysis. Hispanic Linguistics, 1, 115132.Google Scholar
Valdés, G. (2001). Heritage language students: Profiles and possibilities. In Peyton, J.K., Ranard, D.A. & McGinnis, S. (eds), Heritage Languages in America: Preserving a National Resource. Language in Education: Theory and Practice (pp. 3777). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics/Delta Systems.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2009). Abstract categories or limited scope formulae? The case of children”s determiners. Journal of Child Language, 36, 743778.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-Macgregor, M., Leung, I., & Ayed, H. B. (2001). The status of abstract features in interlanguage: Gender and number in L2 Spanish. In Do, A. H.-J. (ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 792802). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2000). Syntactic transfer in a Cantonese–English bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3 (3), 193208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2009). Conditions on cross-linguistic influence in bilingual acquisition: the case of wh-interrogatives. Paper presented at the 7th International Symposium on Bilingualism. Utrecht, Netherlands. July 2009.Google Scholar
Zagona, K. (2002). The syntax of Spanish. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar