Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T16:12:26.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Color and language: Worldwide distribution of Daltonism and distinct words for “blue”

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2005

ANGELA M. BROWN
Affiliation:
College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus
DELWIN T. LINDSEY
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Mansfield

Abstract

A geographical review of the published literature confirmed that red–green color-vision deficiency (Daltonism) is rare near the equator and more prevalent at higher latitudes, as others have reported. A survey of dictionaries of the languages spoken by the populations for which Daltonism data are available confirmed that distinct words for “blue” are also rare near the equator, and more common at higher latitudes. These results were compared to show an even stronger relation between the proportion of languages with a word for “blue” and the prevalence of Daltonism. We believe that the strong correlation between “blue” and Daltonism suggests that an evolutionary, physiological cause for both phenomena.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adam, A. (1980). Polymorphisms of red–green vision in some populations of southern Africa. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 53, 338346.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. & Kay, P. (1969/1991). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley and Los Angeles, California: University of California Press.
Bornstein, M.H. (1973). Color vision and color naming: A psychophysiological hypothesis of cultural difference. Psychological Bulletin 80(4), 257285.Google Scholar
Dutta, P.C. (1966). A review of the inherited defective colour-vision variability and selection relaxation among the Indians. Acta Genetica Basel 16, 327339.Google Scholar
Khérumian, R. & Pickford, R.W. (1959). Hérédité et fréquence des anomalies congénitales du sens chromatique (dyschromatopsies). Paris, France: Vigot Frêres.
Levinson, S. (2000). Yeli dnye and the theory of basic color terms. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 10, 153.Google Scholar
Lindsey, D.T. & Brown, A.M. (2002). Color naming and the phototoxic effects of sunlight on the eye. Psychological Science 13, 506512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
New York Times Atlas of the World (1985). New York: Times Books.
Pokorny, J., Smith, V.C., & Lutze, M. (1987). Aging of the human lens. Applied Optics 26(8), 14371440.Google Scholar
Salzano, F.M. & Neel, J.V. (1976). New data on the vision of South American Indians. Pan American Health Bulletin X, 17.Google Scholar