Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T22:05:29.828Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Category development in early language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Leslie A. Rescorla
Affiliation:
Yale University

Abstract

Developing knowledge of the vehicle, animal, and fruit categories was traced in six children from 1; 0 to 1; 8. Data from mothers' language diaries and from bi-monthly sessions with the children were pooled to analyse the growth, content, and internal structure of the three categories over time. The children developed some grasp of most of the focal concepts in each category, but they made fewer differentiations than adults do. Overextension of a single concept term to encompass a cluster of related referents was common. The frequent discrepancies between comprehension and production of concept terms highlighted the importance of examining both modes. The data showed marked individual differences in style of category acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. JChLang 6. 183200.Google ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L. M. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1976). Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In Morehead, D. M. & Morehead, A. E. (eds), Normal and deficient child language. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Oliver, R. & Greenfleld, P. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, A. F. & Chamberlain, J. (1904). Studies of a child. PedSem 11. 264–91.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Faulkender, P. J., Wright, J. C. & Waldron, A. (1974). Generalized habituation of concept stimuli. ChDev 45. 1002–10.Google ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, S., Perlmutter, M. & Myers, N. (1974). Recall of related and unrelated lists by 2 year olds. JExpChPsych 18. 18.Google Scholar
Greenfleld, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976). Communication and the beginnings of language: the development of semantic structure in one-word speech and beyond. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gruendel, J. M. (1977) Referential extension in early language development. ChDev 48. 1567–76.Google Scholar
Guillaume, P. (1927). Imitation in children. (Trans. Halperin, E. P..) Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. G. (ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology. Potomac, Maryland: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic in the child. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. K. (1939). Speech development of a bilingual child. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. M. (1951) Infant speech (2nd edn.). London: Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Moore, K. C. (1896). Mental development of a child. PsychRev. Monogr. Supp. 1, No. 3.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973 a). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Dev. 38 (12), Serial No. 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973 b). Some evidence for the cognitive primacy of categorization and its functional basis. MPQ 19. 2139.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1980). Overextension in early language development. JChLang 7. 321–35.Google ScholarPubMed
Riccuiti, H. (1965). Object grouping and selective ordering in infants 12–24 months old. MPQ II. 129–48.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ross, G. S. (1980). Concept categorization in 1–2 year olds. DevPsych. 16. 391–6.Google Scholar
Ross, G. S., Nelson, K., Wetstone, H. & Tanouye, E. (in preparation). Concept learning and generalization in toddlers.Google Scholar
Rossi, E. L. & Rossi, S. I. (1965). Concept utilization, serial order and recall in nursery school children. ChDev 36. 771–9.Google Scholar
Thomson, J. R. & Chapman, R. S. (1977). Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: the status of twoyear-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension. JChLang 4. 359–75.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (Trans. Hansfmann, E. & Vakar, G..) Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar