Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:16:11.777Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Communicative intent in late-talking toddlers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Leslie Rescorla*
Affiliation:
Bryn Mawr College
Lisa Merrin
Affiliation:
Tempe School District
*
Department of Psychology, Bryn Mawr College, 101 N. Merion Avenue, Bryn Mawr, PA 19010. Email: lrescorl@brynmawr.edu

Abstract

This research investigated communicative intent in 31 toddlers who were slow to talk and 32 normally developing toddlers matched on SES, age, and nonverbal cognitive ability. Communicative intent was studied during free play, both with the mother and with an unfamiliar examiner. Late talkers had lower rates of communication, initiation, and joint attention, but when total communicativeness was controlled for, they were just as likely to initiate, respond, and maintain joint attention as were their peers. As expected, the late talkers relied more on nonword vocalization, gestures, and gesture/oral combinations than their normally developing peers. Children in both groups initiated much more with the examiner (who was instructed to be passive) than with their mothers. Finally, regression analysis suggested that intake expressive language delay severity was the best predictor of age 3 MLU and IPSyn language outcome in the late talkers. However, after expressive delay severity was accounted for, late talkers who were more interested in initiating communication and sustaining joint attention had worse outcomes than late talkers whose communicative drive was weaker, suggesting that they had a more severe underlying language dysfunction.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1990). Symbolic gesture in normal infants. Child Development,59, 450466.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaoni, L., & Voltcrra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language and Communication. 1 155178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coggins, T., & Carpenter, R. (1981). The communicative intention inventory. Applied Psycholin-guistics, 2, 213234.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Friel-Patti, S. (1983). Mothers’ discourse adjustments to language-impairedand non-language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 360367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Friel-Patti, S. (1984). Mother-child dialogues: A comparison of normal and language impaired children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 17, 1935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, C. E., Siegel, L. S., van der Spuy, H. I. J., Clark, M. L., & Bow, S. J. (1985). The behavioral and linguistic interactions of specifically language-delayed and normal boys withtheir mothers. Child Development, 56, 13891403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1991). The Childes project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mirak, J., & Rescorla, L. (1998). Phonetic skills and vocabulary size in late talkers: Concurrent andpredictive relationships. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R., & Shiffer, M. E. (1991). Communicative initiations in normal and late-talking toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 419431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language intoddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Lee, E. C. (in press). Language impairment in children. In Layton, T. & Watson, L. (Eds.), Early language impairment in children: Vol. I. Nature. New York: Delmar.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Fechnay, T. (1996). Mother-child synchrony and communicative reciprocity in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 200208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L., & Goossens, M. (1992). Symbolic play development in toddlers with expressive specific language impairment (SLI-E). Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 12901302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L., & Ratner, N. B. (1996). Phonetic profiles of toddlers with specific expressive languageimpairment (SLI-E). Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 153165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., Roberts, J., & Dahlsgaard, K. (1997). Late talkers at 2: Outcome at age 3. Journal ofSpeech and Hearing Research, 40, 556566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L., & Schwartz, E. (1990). Outcome of toddlers with expressive language delay. AppliedPsycholinguistics, II, 393407.Google Scholar
Reynell, J. (1977). Reynell Developmental Language Scales (Revised). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). The Index of Productive Syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 14541463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4, 197212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G., & Bryan, A. A. (1989). Communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: Implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 148158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherby, A., Cain, D., Yonclas, D., & Walker, V. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 240252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G., Fischel, J., Lonigan, C., Valdez-Menchaca, M., DeBaryshe, B. D. & Caulfield, M. B. (1988). Verbal interaction in families of normal and expressive-language-delayed chil-dren. Developmental Psychology, 24, 690699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar