Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T01:09:08.228Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

With diversity in mind: Freeing the language sciences from Universal Grammar

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2009

Nicholas Evans
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Research School of Asian and Pacific Studies, Australian National University, ACT 0200, Australianicholas.evans@anu.edu.auhttp://rspas.anu.edu.au/people/personal/evann_ling.php
Stephen C. Levinson
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Wundtlaan 1, NL-6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands; and Radboud University, The Netherlands. stephen.levinson@mpi.nlhttp://www.mpi.nl/Members/StephenLevinson

Abstract

Our response takes advantage of the wide-ranging commentary to clarify some aspects of our original proposal and augment others. We argue against the generative critics of our coevolutionary program for the language sciences, defend the use of close-to-surface models as minimizing cross-linguistic data distortion, and stress the growing role of stochastic simulations in making generalized historical accounts testable. These methods lead the search for general principles away from idealized representations and towards selective processes. Putting cultural evolution central in understanding language diversity makes learning fundamental in the cognition of language: increasingly powerful models of general learning, paired with channelled caregiver input, seem set to manage language acquisition without recourse to any innate “universal grammar.” Understanding why human language has no clear parallels in the animal world requires a cross-species perspective: crucial ingredients are vocal learning (for which there are clear non-primate parallels) and an intention-attributing cognitive infrastructure that provides a universal base for language evolution. We conclude by situating linguistic diversity within a broader trend towards understanding human cognition through the study of variation in, for example, human genetics, neurocognition, and psycholinguistic processing.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, G. D. S. (2007) The Munda verb. Typological perspectives. Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateman, J. (1986a) Tone morphemes and aspect in Iau. Nusa 26:150.Google Scholar
Bateman, J. (1986b) Tone morphemes and status in Iau. Nusa 26:5176.Google Scholar
Bateman, J. (1990a) Iau segmental and tone phonology. Nusa 32:2942.Google Scholar
Bateman, J. (1990b) Pragmatic functions of the tone morphemes and illocutionary force particles in Iau. Nusa 32:128.Google Scholar
Bickel, B. (2009) Typological patterns and hidden diversity. Plenary Talk, 8th Association for Linguistic Typology Conference, Berkeley, CA, July 24, 2009.Google Scholar
Blake, B. J. (1979) Pitta-Pitta. In: Handbook of Australian languages, vol. 1, ed. Dixon, R. M. W. & Blake, B. J., pp. 182242. Australian National University (ANU) Press.Google Scholar
Butcher, A. (2006) Australian Aboriginal languages: Consonant-salient phonologies and the “place-of-articulation imperative”. In: Speech production: Models, phonetics processes and techniques, ed. Harrington, J. M. & Tabain, M., pp. 187210. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Butt, M., Dalrymple, M. & Holloway King, T., eds. (2006) Intelligent linguistic architectures: Variations on themes by Ronald M. Kaplan. CSLI.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2007) Of minds and language. Biolinguistics 1:1009–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., Collins, C. & Edelman, S., eds. (2009) Language universals. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray.Google Scholar
Dryer, M. (2009) On the order of demonstrative, numeral, adjective and noun: An alternative to Cinque. Public Lecture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Linguistics, and University of Leipzig, Institute of Linguistics, May 19, 2009.Google Scholar
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C. & Gray, R. D. (in preparation) Phylogenetic trees reveal lineage specific trends in the evolved structure of language.Google Scholar
Edelman, S. & Christiansen, M. H. (2003) How seriously should we take minimalist syntax? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7.2:6061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, N. (1995b) Current Issues in Australian phonology. In: Handbook of phonological theory, ed. Goldsmith, J., pp. 723–61. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Evans, N. (2004) Experiencer objects in Iwaidjan languages. In: Non-nominative subjects, vol. 1, ed. Peri, B. & Karumuri Venkata, S., pp. 169–92. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, S. & Scharff, C. (2009) FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends in Genetics 25(4):166–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. (2006) The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition 100(1):173215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (2003) Why we are so smart. In: Language in mind, ed. Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S., pp. 195236. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, C. & Wierzbicka, A., eds. (2002) Meaning and universal grammar – theory and empirical findings. 2 volumes. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hale, K. L. (1982) The logic of Damin kinship terminology. In: Languages of kinship in Aboriginal Australia, ed. Heath, J., Merlan, F. & Rumsey, A., pp. 3137. Oceania Linguistic Monographs.Google Scholar
Haun, D. & Call, J. (2009) Great apes' capacities to recognize relational similarity. Cognition 110:147–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haviland, J. B. (1988) “It's my own invention: A comparative grammatical sketch of colonial Tzotzil” and grammatical annotations. In: The great Tzotzil dictionary of Santo Domingo Zinacantán, with grammatical analysis and historical commentary, ed. Laughlin, R. M. & Haviland, J. B., pp. 79121. (Smithsonian Contributions to Anthropology, No. 31). Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. (1994) “Te xa setel xulem” [The buzzards were circling]: Categories of verbal roots in (Zinacantec) Tzotzil. Linguistics 32:691741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, J. B. (submitted) “White-blossomed on bended knee”: Linguistic mediations of nature and culture. Book chapter for Festschrift for Terry Kaufman, ed. Zavala, R. M. & Smith-Stark, T.. Available at: http://anthro.ucsd.edu/~jhaviland/Publications/BLOSSOMEdit.pfd.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1999) Processing complexity and filler-gap dependencies across grammars. Language 75:244–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewitt, B. G. (1979) Aspects of verbal affixation in Abkhaz (Abžui dialect). Transactions of the Philological Society 77(1): 211–38.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. (1960) The origin of speech. Scientific American 203:8996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, C.-R. (1993) Reverse long-distance dependency and functional uncertainty: The interpretation of Mandarin questions. In: Language, information, and computing, ed. Lee, C. & Kang, B. M., pp. 111–20. Thaehaksa.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. (2008) Universals in phonology? The Linguistic Review 25:83187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idsardi, W. J. (2006) A simple proof that optimality theory is computationally intractable. Linguistic Inquiry 37:271–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobsen, W. H. (1979) Noun and verb in Nootkan. In: The Victoria Conference on northwestern languages, Victoria, British Columbia, November 4/5, 1976, ed. Efrat, B. S., pp. 83155. British Columbia Provincial Museum.Google Scholar
Kluender, R. (1992) Deriving island constraints from principles of predication. In: Island constraints, ed. Goodluck, H. & Rochmont, M., pp. 223–58. Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kluender, R. (1998) On the distinction between strong and weak islands: A processing perspective. In: Syntax and semantics, ed. Culicover, P. & McNally, L., pp. 241–79. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kratochvíl, F. (2007) A grammar of Abui. Doctoral dissertation, Leiden University.Google Scholar
Kruspe, N. (2004) A grammar of Semelai. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J. & Feldman, M. W. (1999) Evolutionary consequences of niche construction and their implications for ecology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 96:10242–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. C. (1987) Pragmatics and the grammar of anaphora. Journal of Linguistics 23:379434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2006) Toward an evolutionary biology of language. Belknap/Harvard.Google Scholar
Liljencrants, J. & Lindblom, B. (1972) Numerical simulations of vowel quality systems: The role of perceptual contrast. Language 48:839–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. J. (2002) A thematic guide to optimality theory (Research Surveys in Linguistics). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Molotsi, K. J. (1993) The characteristics of Southern Sotho ideophones. Master's thesis, University of Stellenbosch.Google Scholar
Müller, R.-A. (2009) Language universals in the brain: How linguistic are they? In: Language universals, ed. Christiansen, M., Collins, C. & Edelman, S., pp. 224–52. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mycock, L. (2006) The typology of constituent questions: A lexical-functional grammar analysis of “Wh”-questions. Unpublished Dissertation. University of Manchester.Google Scholar
Papassotiropoulos, A., Stephan, D. A., Huentelman, M. J., Hoerndli, F. J., Craig, D. W., Pearson, J. V., Huynh, K.-D., Brunner, F., Corneveaux, J., Osborne, D., Wollmer, M. A., Aerni, A., Coluccia, D., Hänggi, J., Mondadori, C. R. A., Buchmann, A., Reiman, E. M., Caselli, R. J., Henke, K. & de Quervain, D. J.-F. (2006) Common Kibra alleles are associated with human memory performance. Science 314(5798):475–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pawley, A., Gi, S. P., Majnep, I. S. & Kias, J. (2000) Hunger acts on me: The grammar and semantics of bodily and mental process expressions in Kalam. In: Grammatical analysis: Morphology, syntax and semantics: Studies in honor of Stan Starosta, ed. De Guzman, V. P. & Bender, B. W., pp. 153–85. University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Ramamurti, G. V. (1931) A manual of the So:ra (or Savara) language. Government Press.Google Scholar
Reuer, V. (2004) Book review of Falk, Yehuda N., Lexical-functional grammar – an introduction to parallel constraint-based syntax. Lecture Notes No. 126 (CSLI-LN). Center for the Study of Language and Information, Stanford, 2001, xv+237 pages. Machine Translation 18.4:359–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sag, I., Hofmeister, P. & Snider, N. (2007) Processing complexity in subjacency violations: The complex noun phrase constraint. Chicago Linguistics Society 43(1):219–29.Google Scholar
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I. & Padden, C. (2009) The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Master's thesis, University of Haifa, State University of New York at Stony Brook, and University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Sandler, W. & Lillo-Martin, D. C. (2006) Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, P. (1985) Parts-of-speech systems. In: Language typology and syntactic description, vol. 1: Clause structure, ed. Shopen, T., pp. 361. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K.-E. & Levinson, S. C. (2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106(26):10587–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Valin, R. D. (1998) The acquisition of WH-questions and the mechanisms of language acquisition. In: The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, ed. Tomasello, M., pp. 221–49. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuidema, W. & De Boer, B. (2009) The evolution of combinatorial phonology. Journal of Phonetics 37(2):125–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar