Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T06:56:21.340Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Letter to the Editor: Migrant status, vitamin D and risk of schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2011

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Bourque and colleagues have provided a useful update of the evidence linking migrant status and risk of schizophrenia (Bourque et al. Reference Bourque, van der Ven and Malla2010). The evidence is convincing. What remains for the research community is to generate biologically plausible mechanisms that may underpin this increased risk. Previously, we proposed that low prenatal vitamin D might be a risk factor contributing to the increased risk of schizophrenia in certain migrant groups (McGrath, Reference McGrath1999). Bourque et al. note in their review that this hypothesis might account for part of the increased risk, but suggest that this candidate ‘could scarcely account for the higher risk among lighter-skinned immigrants in some contexts (e.g. Moroccans in The Netherlands) or other groups who moved to warmer climates’.

In fact, there is abundant evidence showing that ‘lighter-skinned immigrants’ such as the Moroccans in the Netherlands are at increased risk of hypovitaminosis D. A detailed systematic review has recently been published on this topic (van der Meer et al. Reference van der Meer, Middelkoop, Boeke and Lips2010). With respect to the relative difference in this exposure between different migrant groups, a population-based study of German children and adolescents (n=10 015) has provided informative relative risks (Hintzpeter et al. Reference Hintzpeter, Scheidt-Nave, Muller, Schenk and Mensink2008). Compared with non-immigrants, those from Africa have the highest adjusted odds ratio for vitamin D deficiency (about seven-fold), followed by migrants from Arab-Islamic countries (about six-fold) and Turkey (about four-fold) (Hintzpeter et al. Reference Hintzpeter, Scheidt-Nave, Muller, Schenk and Mensink2008). Apart from darker skin colour, variables related to dress (e.g. wearing a veil), behaviour (e.g. less outdoor activities) and diet also contribute to an increased risk of vitamin D deficiency in certain minority groups, regardless of skin colour or ethnicity (Rejnmark et al. Reference Rejnmark, Jorgensen, Pedersen, Hansen, Heickendorff, Lauridsen, Mulvad, Siggaard, Skjoldborg, Sorensen, Pedersen and Mosekilde2004; Holick, Reference Holick2007; Lips, Reference Lips2010). Of course, you do not need to be a migrant to have low vitamin D deficiency and insufficiency are prevalent in many nations (Mithal et al. Reference Mithal, Wahl, Bonjour, Burckhardt, Dawson-Hughes, Eisman, El-Hajj Fuleihan, Josse, Lips and Morales-Torres2009).

The original vitamin D hypothesis focused exclusively on prenatal exposures. Animal experiments demonstrate that transient prenatal hypovitaminosis D is associated with persisting changes in brain structure and function, including convergent evidence of altered dopaminergic function (McGrath et al. Reference McGrath, Burne, Feron, Mackay-Sim and Eylesin press). Thus, the hypothesis might account for the second-generation migrants who are at increased risk of developmental vitamin D deficiency, but it cannot explain the first generation migrants who arrive as children or adults. In recent years new evidence has accumulated showing that vitamin D has neuroprotective properties in the post-natal brain (McCann & Ames, Reference McCann and Ames2008). Thus, it is feasible that chronic hypovitaminosis D could leave individuals more vulnerable to subsequent neurobiological insults. For example, migrant groups exposed to both ‘social defeat’ (Selten & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten and Cantor-Graae2005) and hypovitaminosis D may be less able to buffer neurotoxicity related to stress-related mechanisms (Obradovic et al. Reference Obradovic, Gronemeyer, Lutz and Rein2006).

Because vitamin D is safe, cheap, acceptable to the general public, and could help a range of physical health outcomes other than schizophrenia, there is a public health case to undertake exploratory trials of vitamin D in groups at risk of hypovitaminosis D promptly (McGrath, Reference McGrath2010).

Declaration of Interest

None.

The authors reply

We are grateful to John McGrath for his useful literature update and eloquent elaboration on the potential role of vitamin D deficiency in the onset of schizophrenia and related disorders among immigrants.

There is indeed a need to investigate biologically plausible mechanisms that may underlie the excessive incidence of psychotic disorders among migrant and ethnic minority groups. Insights gained from such knowledge may shed light on the onset of psychosis, not only among migrants, but in the general population. Also, any plausible and reasonably testable hypothesis deserves further consideration from the scientific community if it may help us address what has been appropriately termed as ‘a public health tragedy’ (Morgan & Hutchinson, Reference Morgan and Hutchinson2010). One of the most appealing aspects of the vitamin D deficiency hypothesis is that it may be potentially correctable, as highlighted in McGrath's letter.

Although this research avenue may be promising, we would caution against being overly optimistic in explaining the migration and psychosis conundrum based on any single hypothesis. For instance, there are a number of questions that remain unaddressed with the vitamin D hypothesis as currently proposed. Our findings suggest that putative explanatory models should ideally account for the increased risk of psychotic disorders among both first and second generations of immigrants. As acknowledged in the letter, if the increased liability for psychotic disorders among immigrants resulted primarily from a peri-natal or early-life exposure to hypovitaminosis D, it is unclear how this could account for the risk among first-generation immigrants – who appear at higher risk even though they migrated in adulthood. Given the relatively early onset of psychotic disorders (with a peak around 22 years old), there would be only limited exposure to the putative risk factor following immigration. Further evidence on the relationship between age at migration and the risk of psychosis may eventually provide further clues in this respect.

Further considerations include the need to account for increased incidence rates not only among prevailing groups migrating to latitudes less exposed to sunlight, but also among others who moved to similar or even warmer climates (e.g. Finns in Sweden, Russians in Israel or Greenlanders in Denmark). In addition, Asian immigrants in the UK appear to present higher rates of hypovitaminosis D, yet without the substantially higher risk of psychosis observed in immigrants of Caribbean origin (Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson and Murray2010). Without implying causality, it is at least a significant source of concern that the latter group is reportedly the most likely to report instances of racial harassment or discrimination (Karlsen et al. Reference Karlsen, Nazroo and McKenzie2005).

Our review has highlighted considerable variation in the magnitude of the risk across countries, but also within countries (Bourque et al. Reference Bourque, van der Ven and Malla2010). For instance, in The Netherlands, there seems to be a gradient whereby Moroccan immigrants present the highest risk for psychosis, followed by those of Surinamese origin, and lastly those of Turkish origin. It is rather intriguing that the degree of perceived discrimination appears to follow a similar pattern (Veling et al. Reference Veling, Selten, Susser, Laan, Mackenbach and Hoek2007). But perhaps even more striking are the replicated findings of a ‘neighbourhood ethnic density’ phenomenon, whereby the risk of migrant and ethnic minority groups appears to increase as they comprise a lower proportion of the neighbourhood population, in a seemingly dose–response fashion (Boydell et al. Reference Boydell, van Os, McKenzie, Allardyce, Goel, McCreadie and Murray2001; Kirkbride et al. Reference Kirkbride, Morgan, Fearon, Dazzan, Murray and Jones2007; Veling et al. Reference Veling, Susser, van Os, Mackenbach, Selten and Hoek2008). Such patterns of risk defy strictly biological explanations, and indicate that the broader social environment must be considered as we attempt to understand the relationship between migrant status and the risk for psychotic disorders.

Morgan et al. (Reference Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson and Murray2010) have recently suggested a socio-developmental model in an attempt to integrate the expanding evidence for social determinants with the existing knowledge on genetic and neurodevelopmental pathways to psychosis risk factors. It is possible that hypotheses such as vitamin D deficiency (McGrath, Reference McGrath1999; Dealberto, Reference Dealberto2007) or social defeat (Selten & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten and Cantor-Graae2005) may both operate within such a model. Epigenetic mechanisms represent another promising area that may reconcile genetic, biological and socio-environmental risk factors (Peedicayil, Reference Peedicayil2008), as they represent a plausible model according to which various environmental exposures – including adverse social experiences – may translate into differential gene expression.

While some biological risk factors may be reliably measured through blood samples, socio-environmental exposures may not be yet readily identifiable or reliably measured with prevailing psychiatric research methods. McGrath has recently argued that shared research endeavours and cross-disciplinary exchanges between schizophrenia epidemiology and basic neuroscience would be of much benefit to both fields (McGrath & Richards, Reference McGrath and Richards2009). We are very much in agreement with this statement, but we would like to add that there is also a call to better integrate the knowledge and approaches of social sciences if the scientific community is to make significant advances in the complex relationship between migration, ethnicity and psychosis, a public health problem which has lagged behind for too long at the expense of vulnerable populations.

References

Bourque, F, van der Ven, E, Malla, A (2010). A meta-analysis of the risk for psychotic disorders among first- and second-generation immigrants. Psychological Medicine. Published online: 21 July 2010. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001406.Google ScholarPubMed
Hintzpeter, B, Scheidt-Nave, C, Muller, MJ, Schenk, L, Mensink, GB (2008). Higher prevalence of vitamin D deficiency is associated with immigrant background among children and adolescents in Germany. Journal of Nutrition 138, 14821490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holick, M (2007). Vitamin D deficiency. New England Journal of Medicine 357, 266281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lips, P (2010). Worldwide status of vitamin D nutrition. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 121, 297300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCann, JC, Ames, BN (2008). Is there convincing biological or behavioral evidence linking vitamin D deficiency to brain dysfunction? FASEB Journal 22, 9821001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J (1999). Hypothesis: is low prenatal vitamin D a risk-modifying factor for schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Research 40, 173177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J (2010). Is it time to trial vitamin D supplements for the prevention of schizophrenia? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 121, 321324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, JJ, Burne, TH, Feron, F, Mackay-Sim, A, Eyles, DW (in press). Developmental vitamin D deficiency and risk of schizophrenia: a ten-year update. Schizophrenia Bulletin.Google Scholar
Mithal, A, Wahl, DA, Bonjour, JP, Burckhardt, P, Dawson-Hughes, B, Eisman, JA, El-Hajj Fuleihan, G, Josse, RG, Lips, P, Morales-Torres, J; IOF Committee of Scientific Advisors (CSA) Nutrition Working Group (2009). Global vitamin D status and determinants of hypovitaminosis D. Osteoporosis International 20, 18071820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obradovic, D, Gronemeyer, H, Lutz, B, Rein, T (2006). Cross-talk of vitamin D and glucocorticoids in hippocampal cells. Journal of Neurochemistry 96, 500509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rejnmark, L, Jorgensen, ME, Pedersen, MB, Hansen, JC, Heickendorff, L, Lauridsen, AL, Mulvad, G, Siggaard, C, Skjoldborg, H, Sorensen, TB, Pedersen, EB, Mosekilde, L (2004). Vitamin D insufficiency in Greenlanders on a westernized fare: ethnic differences in calcitropic hormones between Greenlanders and Danes. Calcified Tissue International 74, 255263.Google ScholarPubMed
Selten, JP, Cantor-Graae, E (2005). Social defeat: risk factor for schizophrenia? British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 101102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Meer, IM, Middelkoop, BJ, Boeke, AJ, Lips, P (2010). Prevalence of vitamin D deficiency among Turkish, Moroccan, Indian and sub-Sahara African populations in Europe and their countries of origin: an overview. Osteoporosis International. Published online: 12 May 2010. doi: 10.1007/s0019801012791.Google ScholarPubMed