Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:01:26.674Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ageing, social class and common mental disorders: longitudinal evidence from three cohorts in the West of Scotland

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 May 2010

M. J. Green*
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council, Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, Glasgow, UK
M. Benzeval
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council, Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, Glasgow, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Mr M. J. Green, MRC Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, 4 Lilybank Gardens, GlasgowG12 8RZ, UK. (Email: michael-g@sphsu.mrc.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Understanding how common mental disorders such as anxiety and depression vary with socio-economic circumstances as people age can help to identify key intervention points. However, much research treats these conditions as a single disorder when they differ significantly in terms of their disease burden. This paper examines the socio-economic pattern of anxiety and depression separately and longitudinally to develop a better understanding of their disease burden for key social groups at different ages.

Method

The Twenty-07 Study has followed 4510 respondents from three cohorts in the West of Scotland for 20 years and 3846 respondents had valid data for these analyses. Hierarchical repeated-measures models were used to investigate the relationship between age, social class and the prevalence of anxiety and depression over time measured as scores of 8 or more out of 21 on the relevant subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS).

Results

Social class differences in anxiety and depression widened with age. For anxiety there was a nonlinear decrease in prevalence with age, decreasing more slowly for those from manual classes compared to non-manual, whereas for depression there was a non-linear increase in prevalence with age, increasing more quickly for those from manual classes compared to non-manual. This relationship is robust to cohort, period and attrition effects.

Conclusions

The more burdensome disorder of depression occurs more frequently at ages where socio-economic inequalities in mental health are greatest, representing a ‘double jeopardy’ for older people from a manual class.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010 The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/>. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.

Introduction

Common mental disorders such as anxiety and depression have been estimated to account for substantial proportions of the burden of disease in developed countries, and the estimated burden of these conditions varies between age groups (Murray & Lopez, Reference Murray and Lopez1996; Mathers et al. Reference Mathers, Lopez, Murray, Lopez, Mathers, Ezzati, Jamison and Murray2006). Understanding the demographic patterning of disease burden is important for strategic health planning (Lopez et al. Reference Lopez, Mathers, Ezzati, Jamison, Murray, Lopez, Mathers, Ezzati, Jamison and Murray2006), and as tackling socio-economic inequalities in health is a stated policy goal, both in the UK and internationally (Marmot et al. Reference Marmot, Friel, Bell, Houweling and Taylor2008; DOH, 2009), differences in disease burden between socio-economic groups are of particular interest. Although research often demonstrates associations between socio-economic disadvantage and psychological distress, it is not always clear how these vary with age. However, improving understanding of this age patterning would be valuable in assessing the needs of an ageing population, especially as the Royal College of Psychiatrists has recently suggested that the UK currently provides fewer mental health services for those over 65 than for younger people (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2009). An additional issue with current evidence is that measures of distress often group anxiety and depression together. However, these disorders differ in terms of their disease burden, so it is important to understand the potential differences in patterning between them.

Longitudinal research has shown relationships between better mental health and higher occupational classes (Marmot et al. Reference Marmot, Shipley, Brunner and Hemingway2001; Power et al. Reference Power, Stansfeld, Matthews, Manor and Hope2002; Sacker & Wiggins, Reference Sacker and Wiggins2002; Stansfeld et al. Reference Stansfeld, Head, Fuhrer, Wardle and Cattell2003; Singh-Manoux et al. Reference Singh-Manoux, Ferrie, Chandola and Marmot2004; Chandola et al. Reference Chandola, Ferrie, Sacker and Marmot2007), higher levels of income or education (Kim & Durden, Reference Kim and Durden2007; Beard et al. Reference Beard, Tracy, Vlahov and Galea2008), and advantages in childhood socio-economic position (Gilman et al. Reference Gilman, Kawachi, Fitzmaurice and Buka2002; Power et al. Reference Power, Stansfeld, Matthews, Manor and Hope2002; Singh-Manoux et al. Reference Singh-Manoux, Ferrie, Chandola and Marmot2004; Tiffin et al. Reference Tiffin, Pearce and Parker2005; Wiles et al. Reference Wiles, Peters, Leon and Lewis2005; Mensah & Hobcraft, Reference Mensah and Hobcraft2008). Cross-sectional evidence also suggests that relationships between socio-economic variables and psychological disorder can vary, or even strengthen, as people age (Miech & Shanahan, Reference Miech and Shanahan2000; Fryers et al. Reference Fryers, Melzer and Jenkins2003). However, the age dependency of this relationship has rarely been made explicit in longitudinal research, with a tendency either to simply adjust for age (Marmot et al. Reference Marmot, Shipley, Brunner and Hemingway2001; Stansfeld et al. Reference Stansfeld, Head, Fuhrer, Wardle and Cattell2003; Singh-Manoux et al. Reference Singh-Manoux, Ferrie, Chandola and Marmot2004; Wiggins et al. Reference Wiggins, Schofield, Sacker, Head and Bartley2004) or to only consider psychological distress as an outcome at one time point for participants of equivalent age (Tiffin et al. Reference Tiffin, Pearce and Parker2005; Wiles et al. Reference Wiles, Peters, Leon and Lewis2005; Mensah & Hobcraft, Reference Mensah and Hobcraft2008). Insofar as variation by age has been addressed explicitly in the literature, the results have been inconsistent: some show inequalities widening with age and others show them narrowing (Sacker & Wiggins, Reference Sacker and Wiggins2002; Chandola et al. Reference Chandola, Ferrie, Sacker and Marmot2007; Kim & Durden, Reference Kim and Durden2007).

In addition to the ambiguity over age patterning, the outcome measures often used to show relationships between common mental disorders and socio-economic circumstances do not discriminate between anxiety and depression. Such measures confound the prevalence of the two disorders, making it more difficult to discern where the burden of disease is greatest, and may not be offering a clear picture of the inequalities between groups. For example, depression has been shown to be more disabling and more consistently associated with mortality than anxiety (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Monson, Olivier, Sobol and Leighton1987; Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Sanderson, Slade and Issakidis2000; Eaton et al. Reference Eaton, Martins, Nestadt, Bienvenu, Clarke and Alexandre2008), so a difference between groups in the prevalence of depression will mean more in terms of disease burden than a similar difference between groups for anxiety. There is some evidence that age and socio-economic effects can differ by disorder (for examples see Stansfeld et al. Reference Stansfeld, Head and Marmot1998; Beekman et al. Reference Beekman, de Beurs, van Balkom, Deeg, van Dyck and van Tilburg2000; Vink et al. Reference Vink, Aartsen and Schoevers2008) and improved understanding of such differences would help to clarify the social patterning of disease burden. The aim of this paper was therefore to extend previous work by using longitudinal data from three cohorts to examine the relationship between age, socio-economic status and the prevalence of anxiety and depression.

Method

Design and setting

Data for this paper were taken from the Twenty-07 Study (for full details see Benzeval et al. Reference Benzeval, Der, Ellaway, Hunt, Sweeting, West and Macintyre2009), which was established as a two-stage stratified random sample of 4510 people from three age cohorts (born around 1932, 1952 and 1972) living in the Central Clydeside Conurbation in the West of Scotland. The baseline interviews were carried out in 1987/88 when respondents were aged approximately 15, 35 and 55 years, and there were four repeat visits in 1990/2, 1995/7, 2000/4 and 2007/8, providing 20 years of follow-up for each cohort and covering 60 years of the lifespan. Baseline respondents have been shown to be representative of the general population of the sampled area (Der, Reference Der1998). The Twenty-07 Study is particularly well placed to address the questions under consideration as it includes the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), which was designed to discriminate between disorders (Zigmond & Snaith, Reference Zigmond and Snaith1983).

Measures

The HADS was administered at each of the four follow-up visits. It has been used in clinical and general population settings, and correlates well with interview-based measures and other screening questionnaires that identify psychiatric distress (for a review see Bjelland et al. Reference Bjelland, Dahl, Haug and Neckelmann2002). The HADS has two subscales, one for anxiety and one for depression, and each has seven items scored on a four-point scale between 0 and 3, creating a maximum score of 21 on each subscale. For this analysis, if only one or two items on a subscale were missing, the score was calculated as the mean of valid responses multiplied by seven (Roness et al. Reference Roness, Mykletun and Dahl2005). Total scores of 8 or more on either subscale have been shown to have sensitivity and specificity of approximately 80% for finding clinical cases. Although this validation was mostly within clinical settings, a community survey also showed similar values (Bjelland et al. Reference Bjelland, Dahl, Haug and Neckelmann2002) and so this threshold was used to define cases.

Table 1 shows prevalence rates for disorder at each wave. The categories shown are not mutually exclusive, that is anxiety cases and depression cases were defined without regard to co-morbidity. Anxiety cases were more prevalent than depression cases, and comparing the rates for each disorder with those for combined anxiety and depression shows that depression was mostly only present in combination with anxiety but that the reverse was not true of anxiety. Cross-sectional normative data for the HADS in the UK has shown similar prevalence rates (Crawford et al. Reference Crawford, Henry, Crombie and Taylor2001); in the normative data 33% had scores of 8 or more on the anxiety subscale and 11.4% had scores of 8 or more for depression, whereas in the fourth wave of Twenty-07 (the closest time point for comparison), the respective figures were 35.7% and 13.2%.

Table 1. Distribution of common mental disorders and baseline characteristics across the study waves

n.a., Not applicable.

a Missingness is high in wave 3 because a portion of the sample only received a postal questionnaire that did not include the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) instrument.

b As this column represents only the modelled data and person-years with missing data were not included in the models, there are no missing values here.

Socio-economic disadvantage was measured by baseline occupational class, coded according to the Registrar General's 1980 classification (Office of Population Censuses and Surveys, 1980) for head of household's current or previous occupation. In multiple person households, the head was defined as the husband (or father for the 1970s cohort), and if they did not have an occupation then the wife/mother's was used. Social class has been split into a dichotomous variable comparing manual (III manual, IV and V) to non-manual classes (I, II and III non-manual). To keep estimates for the other parameters neutral (e.g. Sacker et al. Reference Sacker, Clarke, Wiggins and Bartley2005), gender was coded −0.5 for men and 0.5 for women, and age, measured as a continuous variable, was centred on its mean (46.3 years). Dummy variables for cohort (reference: 1950s cohort) and study wave (reference: wave 2) were used to investigate cohort and period effects. A variable representing the number of missed waves ranging from 0 to 3 was also created to examine the effects of sample attrition.

The distribution of respondents at each wave according to these basic characteristics is displayed in the lower part of Table 1. This shows that the modelled data (final column) were reasonably representative of the baseline sample in terms of gender, cohort and occupational class.

Statistical methods

Hierarchical repeated-measures models were used; these take account of the clustered nature of the data and also adjust for non-response if the data are missing at random (Clarke & Hardy, Reference Clarke, Hardy, Pickles, Maughan and Wadsworth2007). Data were included in the analysis for each wave in which respondents participated and had a valid score on both HADS subscales. Logistic models were constructed in MLwiN version 2.02 (Rasbash et al. Reference Rasbash, Charlton, Brown, Healy and Cameron2005) with three levels: measurement points (level 1, n=10629), nested within individuals (level 2, n=3846), nested within primary sampling units (level 3, n=62). Initially, the coefficients for age were allowed to vary at the individual level (a random slope model), but there was no evidence of complex variation at this level and so the more parsimonious random intercept models were used. Models were also initially attempted with second-order penalized quasi-likelihood (PQL) estimation, but given the low numbers of depression cases some models would not converge and therefore, for consistency, all models were estimated using first-order marginal quasi-likelihood estimation (MQL). Lowering the threshold for depression caseness gave enough cases for second-order PQL estimation but did not materially change the findings; therefore, as the threshold for depression caseness was thought to be appropriate, the first-order MQL models were used (details available from the authors on request).

Three main sets of models were constructed. First, for comparison with other literature, caseness for either anxiety or depression, that is a non-discriminatory measure of disorder, was modelled against age, sex and baseline social class, and all possible interactions between age, class and sex were tested. Non-linear age terms were used to examine how the shape of the trajectory varied as people age. Second, similar models were constructed separately for anxiety and depression (although with co-morbid cases included in both instances). Third, sensitivity analyses were conducted to explore whether the observed trajectories were robust to period, cohort and attrition effects.

Three other modelling variations were also tested but are not presented. First, models were repeated using a time-varying social class variable; that is, rather than using baseline class, the class measurement from the previous wave was used at each measurement point (or the most recent wave prior to that if it was missing). Second, the models were repeated using the HADS subscale scores as continuous outcome measures. The results were very similar to the main models in both cases and so, for brevity, only the logistic models using baseline class are shown. Third, all models were also repeated for combined anxiety and depression but, as depression rarely occurred without concurrent anxiety (see Table 1), the results were almost identical to those for depression and are not shown (details available from the authors on request).

Results

Figure 1 shows the predicted probabilities (from the fixed part of the model) and 95% confidence intervals for overall mental distress, that is caseness on either the anxiety or the depression subscale. The age trajectories were nonlinear, with quadratic terms offering significant improvement over the linear model. For those from non-manual classes, the probability of disorder declined with age, with the rate of decline increasing steadily from approximately age 35, whereas for those from manual classes the trajectory for disorder was more of an inverse U-shape with a peak probability of disorder in the late 40s. The difference in mental disorder prevalence between those from non-manual and manual classes increased significantly as respondents aged. Females were more likely to experience disorder across all ages, but no gender interactions with age or social class were evident.

Fig. 1. Age trajectories in common mental disorders by baseline social class and adjusted for gender.

The results from comparable models for each disorder examined separately are displayed in Fig. 2, and the odds ratios for the various parameters in these models can be found in Table 2. Again, age trajectories were non-linear for each disorder, with quadratic terms offering significant model improvement over a linear relationship. The probability of anxiety (Fig. 2 a) was fairly stable with no significant class difference until approximately age 45, at which point the prevalence began to decline, but the decline, representing psychological improvement, was steeper for those in non-manual classes than for those in manual classes. The probability of depression, however (Fig. 2 b), increased steadily from a relatively low prevalence in adolescence, before levelling out somewhat in older age. The prevalence for depression increased more quickly with age for those in manual classes than for those in non-manual classes, with the difference becoming significant around the age of 30. In all models the peak probability of disorder was lower, and at younger ages, for those in non-manual classes than for those in manual classes.

Fig. 2. Disorder-specific age trajectories by social class and adjusted for gender.

Table 2. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for common mental disorders: sensitivity analyses

n.s., The variable did not significantly improve the model and was left out.

a Variables are defined as follows: age is centred on the mean value of 46.3 years; sex is centred on 0 (0.5=female, −0.5=male); for manual, non-manual is the reference category; for the 1970s and 1930s cohorts it is the 1950s cohort; for the 95–97, 00–04 and 07–08 waves it is the 90–92 wave; and missed waves is the number of waves missed ranging from 0 to 3.

b To make odds ratios easier to interpret, age squared was divided by 100 before being entered into the models.

Women were more likely than men to experience anxiety and depression, irrespective of age or social class, but there was also a gender interaction with baseline class for anxiety (see odds ratios in Table 2). This resulted in a wider class difference for women covering a greater portion of the lifecourse (i.e. the confidence intervals separate at earlier ages, around 35 years), whereas class differences in anxiety for men only became significant at older ages (around 60 years). Modelling anxiety for men and women separately offered similar results (not shown). There were no gender interactions evident for depression.

Sensitivity analyses were conducted to ascertain the robustness of the models portrayed in Fig. 2; the results are shown in Table 2. The first column for each disorder contains the odds ratios for the models in Fig. 2, and the next two columns show separate models adjusting for cohort and period effects respectively. Separate models were constructed here because age, cohort and period effects cannot all simultaneously be adjusted for in the same model (Glenn, Reference Glenn2005). In general, including either period or cohort dummies had little impact on the age coefficients, which supports their interpretation as genuine age effects (Hoeymans et al. Reference Hoeymans, Feskens, Van Den Bos and Kromhout1997; Sacker & Wiggins, Reference Sacker and Wiggins2002). In addition, significant main effects of cohort and period were observed for anxiety. Other things being equal, anxiety was more likely in the 1930s cohort and less likely in the 1970s cohort than in the 1950s, and was less likely in the fourth and fifth wave of the study than in the second wave. There were no significant cohort or period effects evident for depression.

The final column for each disorder in Table 2 shows adjustment for the number of missed waves to assess the effect of drop-out on the observed associations (Sacker & Wiggins, Reference Sacker and Wiggins2002). Adding a variable for drop-out had little influence on the other parameters. However, there were significant main effects, indicating that those who missed waves were more likely to be cases for either disorder when they did participate and there was an interaction with age for anxiety, but no interactions with class or gender. This implies that attrition may have caused some underestimation of disorder prevalence, some overestimation of the age gradient in anxiety, but that attrition is unlikely to have had any effect upon the observed class differences.

Discussion

Distinguishing between anxiety and depression in this paper demonstrates that the age trajectories for these disorders follow opposite directions; the probability of anxiety decreases with age whereas depression becomes more probable. This is in accordance with some previous findings (Beekman et al. Reference Beekman, de Beurs, van Balkom, Deeg, van Dyck and van Tilburg2000; Vink et al. Reference Vink, Aartsen and Schoevers2008), although usually an age trend has been found for one disorder and not the other. Social class differences increased with age and indicate, for those in manual classes compared to those in non-manual, slower improvement with age for anxiety and more rapid decrement with age for depression. Trajectories for combined anxiety and depression were also modelled and were found to be almost identical to those for depression, and hence for simplicity are not presented here. Overall, these results show that the difference between manual and non-manual classes is not significant at younger ages but emerges, becoming significant, as it increases in magnitude with age. This supports previous work indicating the potential age-dependency of socio-economic effects on mental health (Miech & Shanahan, Reference Miech and Shanahan2000; Fryers et al. Reference Fryers, Melzer and Jenkins2003), and the findings of Chandola et al. (Reference Chandola, Ferrie, Sacker and Marmot2007) that class differences in mental health increased with age. Sacker & Wiggins (Reference Sacker and Wiggins2002) observed a contradictory pattern, where the socio-economic inequality narrowed with age, but only when modelling for age and cohort, not when comparing age and period effects, so the difference may be attributable to a secular trend.

The different age trajectories observed for anxiety and depression give a clearer understanding of the patterning of disease burden than has been shown previously. First, those who are older were found to be at an increased risk of depression, which has a greater disease burden than anxiety with respect to impairment or mortality. The greater burden associated with this rise in the likelihood of depression at older age is exacerbated by the fact that depression was, in most cases, combined with anxiety. Combined anxiety and depression has been found to show greater risks for both impairment and suicide than for cases of either disorder alone (Wittchen et al. Reference Wittchen, Beesdo, Bittner and Goodwin2003). Second, those who are older and from a manual class experience a ‘double jeopardy’; not only are they at a greater risk of a more burdensome disorder (i.e. depression) than younger people but they are also more likely than those of a similar age from non-manual classes to experience either anxiety or depression. These findings are especially important in the UK, where provision of mental health services for those aged 65 and over is less comprehensive than for younger people, and 84.1% of those with depression in this older age group are receiving no treatment (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2009). This suggests that the provision is lowest, or at least lacking, where there are both the greatest needs and the greatest socio-economic inequalities. Knowledge of these patterns could help to address this imbalance by informing resource allocation for treatment in mental health services and by identifying the people who are disadvantaged and older as a key target group for interventions to prevent mental disorder.

The finding that socio-economic differences in mental disorder widen as people age for both anxiety and depression can be interpreted in the context of stress theory (Thoits, Reference Thoits, Horwitz and Scheid1999), which suggests that groups with high levels of stressors and low levels of coping resources, such as those of disadvantaged socio-economic status, may be more at risk for mental disorders. For example, social support has been found to be less prevalent among more disadvantaged groups (Turner & Marino, Reference Turner and Marino1994; Huurre et al. Reference Huurre, Eerola, Rahkonen and Aro2007), and variations in stress have been shown to explain some of the socio-economic variation in depression (Turner et al. Reference Turner, Wheaton and Lloyd1995). The divergent age trajectories observed here may be caused by the accumulation of coping resources among those with more advantaged socio-economic status as people age (Ross & Wu, 1996; Kim & Durden, Reference Kim and Durden2007), by the accumulation of stressful exposure among disadvantaged groups as people get older (Aldwin & Stokols, Reference Aldwin and Stokols1988), or by some combination of the two. A limitation of stress theory is that it is not specific to particular disorders (Thoits, Reference Thoits, Horwitz and Scheid1999), but these findings suggest that this may be justified: the socio-economic difference widens with age for both disorders. This could be because a common factor, varying with age and socio-economic status, is associated with both anxiety and depression, but it could also be the case that different stressors and/or resources are involved in creating the effect for each disorder. Identification of a common factor would be particularly valuable because that might represent a means of effective intervention for both anxiety and depression.

The 60-year age range covered by this 20-year follow-up of three cohorts has allowed ageing and socio-economic effects on psychiatric morbidity to be examined across a broad portion of the lifespan while maintaining an advantage over cross-sectional research in that period and cohort effects could be explored in sensitivity analyses. The longitudinal data also allowed social class to be examined at different points in time, but this did not affect the results. It has been suggested that cohort effects may confound this type of analysis as older cohorts are less comfortable in reporting psychological symptoms (Aldwin et al. Reference Aldwin, Spiro, Levenson and Bossé1989). However the trajectories reported here were found to be robust to cohort effects and, if anything, the oldest cohort was more likely to report anxiety symptoms controlling for age. Seedat et al. (Reference Seedat, Scott, Angermeyer, Berglund, Bromet, Brugha, Demyttenaere, de Girolamo, Haro, Jin, Karam, Kovess-Masfety, Levinson, Medina Mora, Ono, Ormel, Pennell, Posada-Villa, Sampson, Williams and Kessler2009) found interactions between gender and cohort in a large international study, such that gender differences in depression levels were smaller in more recent cohorts. Similar interactions between cohort and gender were not observed here, nor were any between cohort and social class, although this may have been due to a lack of power to detect such complex interactions.

These analyses addressed the possible effects of attrition bias by including data up to the point at which a respondent drops out and using likelihood estimators. The residual effect of drop-out was examined by including a count of missing waves in sensitivity models. Although this suggested, consistent with other research (Mirowsky & Reynolds, Reference Mirowsky and Reynolds2000), that those who missed waves may have had higher levels of disorder, the other parameters were largely unaffected and thus it is unlikely that drop-out could explain the class differences or the age trends observed.

There are some limitations to these findings, however; although the Twenty-07 study covers a wide age range, there is some evidence that the age gradient in psychiatric morbidity is steepest beyond the age of 70 (Grundy & Sloggett, Reference Grundy and Sloggett2003; Nguyen & Zonderman, Reference Nguyen and Zonderman2006). This age group is only represented here by the last measurement point from the oldest cohort and thus we cannot assess whether people beyond this age have a steeper psychiatric gradient than suggested. One important caveat in relation to the age trends reported here is that, although adjustment for cohort and period effects suggests the age trends are genuine, the results are representative of the individual experiences of the three age cohorts rather than of continuous ageing of individuals across the whole of the lifespan covered.

In relation to the measure of mental disorder, it is important to note that HADS scores do not represent clinical diagnoses of anxiety or depression, so a case might not necessarily require specialist help, but the raised symptomatology measured, even if subclinical, does still represent a disease burden in the community.

Finally, the predictions of disorder at any of the measurement occasions were not adjusted for levels of disorder at any previous measurement occasion. These analyses refer to prevalence only, and may therefore have combined or confounded incidence and individual episode duration (for discussion in relation to physical health, see Dupre, Reference Dupre2007; Taylor, Reference Taylor2008). Future work should examine relationships between socio-economic circumstances and the progression of symptoms in more depth.

In conclusion, this analysis has examined how socio-economic differences in anxiety and depression vary with age, without confounding the prevalence of the two disorders by combining symptoms of each into a single measure. This provides a clearer understanding of the social patterning of disease burden. Socio-economic inequalities in the prevalence of common mental disorders increase with age, as does the overall prevalence of the more burdensome disorder of depression, representing a ‘double jeopardy’ for those who are older and from a manual class.

Acknowledgements

The West of Scotland Twenty-07 Study is funded by the UK Medical Research Council (MRC) (WBS U.1300.80.001.00001) and the data were originally collected by the MRC Social and Public Health Sciences Unit. M. J. Green and M. Benzeval are funded by the MRC (WBS U.1300.00.006.00005.01). We are grateful to all of the participants in the study, and to the survey staff and research nurses who carried it out. We also thank A. Leyland for statistical advice, and the anonymous referees who gave helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Aldwin, CM, Stokols, D (1988). The effects of environmental change on individuals and groups: some neglected issues in stress research. Journal of Environmental Psychology 8, 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aldwin, CM, Spiro, A III, Levenson, MR, Bossé, R (1989). Longitudinal findings from the Normative Aging Study: 1. Does mental health change with age? Psychology and Aging 4, 295306.Google Scholar
Andrews, G, Sanderson, K, Slade, T, Issakidis, C (2000). Why does the burden of disease persist? Relating the burden of anxiety and depression to effectiveness of treatment. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 78, 446454.Google Scholar
Beard, JR, Tracy, M, Vlahov, D, Galea, S (2008). Trajectory and socioeconomic predictors of depression in a prospective study of residents of New York City. Annals of Epidemiology 18, 235243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beekman, ATF, de Beurs, E, van Balkom, AJLM, Deeg, DJH, van Dyck, R, van Tilburg, W (2000). Anxiety and depression in later life: co-occurrence and communality of risk factors. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 8995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benzeval, M, Der, G, Ellaway, A, Hunt, K, Sweeting, H, West, P, Macintyre, S (2009). Cohort Profile: West of Scotland Twenty-07 Study: Health in the Community. International Journal of Epidemiology 38, 12151223.Google Scholar
Bjelland, I, Dahl, AA, Haug, TT, Neckelmann, D (2002). The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: an updated literature review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 52, 6977.Google Scholar
Chandola, T, Ferrie, J, Sacker, A, Marmot, M (2007). Social inequalities in self reported health in early old age: follow-up of prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal 334, 990993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, P, Hardy, R (2007). Methods for handling missing data. In Epidemiological Methods in Life Course Research (ed. Pickles, A., Maughan, B. and Wadsworth, M.), pp. 157180. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Crawford, JR, Henry, JD, Crombie, C, Taylor, EP (2001). Brief report. Normative data for the HADS from a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 40, 429434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Der, G (1998). A comparison of the West of Scotland Twenty-07 study sample and the 1991 census SARs. Working Paper No. 60. MRC Medical Sociology Unit: Glasgow.Google Scholar
DOH (2009). Tackling health inequalities: 10 years on. A review of developments in tackling health inequalities in England over the last 10 years. Department of Health: London.Google Scholar
Dupre, ME (2007). Educational differences in age-related patterns of disease: reconsidering the cumulative disadvantage and age-as-leveler hypotheses. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 48, 115.Google Scholar
Eaton, WW, Martins, SS, Nestadt, G, Bienvenu, OJ, Clarke, D, Alexandre, P (2008). The burden of mental disorders. Epidemiologic Reviews 30, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fryers, T, Melzer, D, Jenkins, R (2003). Social inequalities and the common mental disorders: a systematic review of the evidence. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 38, 229237.Google Scholar
Gilman, SE, Kawachi, I, Fitzmaurice, GM, Buka, SL (2002). Socioeconomic status in childhood and the lifetime risk of major depression. International Journal of Epidemiology 31, 359367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, ND (2005). Cohort Analysis. Sage: Thousand Oaks.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grundy, E, Sloggett, A (2003). Health inequalities in the older population: the role of personal capital, social resources and socio-economic circumstances. Social Science and Medicine 56, 935947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoeymans, N, Feskens, EJM, Van Den Bos, GAM, Kromhout, D (1997). Age, time, and cohort effects on functional status and self-rated health in elderly men. American Journal of Public Health 87, 16201625.Google Scholar
Huurre, T, Eerola, M, Rahkonen, O, Aro, H (2007). Does social support affect the relationship between socioeconomic status and depression? A longitudinal study from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders 100, 5564.Google Scholar
Kim, J, Durden, E (2007). Socioeconomic status and age trajectories of health. Social Science and Medicine 65, 24892502.Google Scholar
Lopez, AD, Mathers, CD, Ezzati, M, Jamison, DT, Murray, CJL (2006). Measuring the global burden of disease and risk factors, 1990–2001. In Global Burden of Disease and Risk Factors (ed. Lopez, A. D., Mathers, C. D., Ezzati, M., Jamison, D. T. and Murray, C. J. L.), pp. 114. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Marmot, M, Friel, S, Bell, R, Houweling, TAJ, Taylor, S (2008). Closing the gap in a generation: health equity through action on the social determinants of health. Lancet 372, 16611669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marmot, M, Shipley, M, Brunner, E, Hemingway, H (2001). Relative contribution of early life and adult socioeconomic factors to adult morbidity in the Whitehall II study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 55, 301307.Google Scholar
Mathers, CD, Lopez, AD, Murray, CJL (2006). The burden of disease and mortality by condition: data, methods and results for 2001. In Global Burden of Disease and Risk Factors (ed. Lopez, A. D., Mathers, C. D., Ezzati, M., Jamison, D. T. and Murray, C. J. L.), pp. 45–240. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Mensah, FK, Hobcraft, J (2008). Childhood deprivation, health and development: associations with adult health in the 1958 and 1970 British prospective birth cohort studies. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 62, 599606.Google Scholar
Miech, RA, Shanahan, MJ (2000). Socioeconomic status and depression over the life course. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 41, 162176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirowsky, J, Reynolds, JR (2000). Age, depression, and attrition in the National Survey of Families and Households. Sociological Methods Research 28, 476504.Google Scholar
Murphy, JM, Monson, RR, Olivier, DC, Sobol, AM, Leighton, AH (1987). Affective disorders and mortality: a general population study. Archives of General Psychiatry 44, 473480.Google Scholar
Murray, CJL, Lopez, AD (eds) (1996). The Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment of Mortality and Disability for Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990 and Projected to 2020. Harvard University School of Public Health on behalf of the World Health Organization and The World Bank: Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Nguyen, HT, Zonderman, AB (2006). Relationship between age and aspects of depression: consistency and reliability across two longitudinal studies. Psychology and Aging 21, 119126.Google Scholar
Office of Population Censuses and Surveys (1980). Classification of Occupations. HMSO: London.Google Scholar
Power, C, Stansfeld, SA, Matthews, S, Manor, O, Hope, S (2002). Childhood and adulthood risk factors for socio-economic differentials in psychological distress: evidence from the 1958 British birth cohort. Social Science and Medicine 55, 19892004.Google Scholar
Rasbash, J, Charlton, C, Brown, WJ, Healy, M, Cameron, B (2005). MLwiN Version 2.02. Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol: UK.Google Scholar
Roness, A, Mykletun, A, Dahl, AA (2005). Help-seeking behaviour in patients with anxiety disorder and depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 111, 5158.Google Scholar
Ross, CE, Wu, C-L (1996). Education, age, and the cumulative advantage in health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 37, 104120.Google Scholar
Royal College of Psychiatrists (2009). The need to tackle age discrimination in mental health. A compendium of evidence (www.rcpsych.ac.uk/pdf/Royal%20College%20of%20Psychiatrists%20-%20The%20Need%20to%20Tackle%20Age%20Discrimination%20in%20Mental%20Health%20Services%20-%20Oct09.pdf). Accessed 28 October 2009.Google Scholar
Sacker, A, Clarke, P, Wiggins, RD, Bartley, M (2005). Social dynamics of health inequalities: a growth curve analysis of aging and self assessed health in the British household panel survey 1991–2001. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59, 495501.Google Scholar
Sacker, A, Wiggins, RD (2002). Age-period-cohort effects on inequalities in psychological distress, 1981–2000. Psychological Medicine 32, 977990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seedat, S, Scott, KM, Angermeyer, MC, Berglund, P, Bromet, EJ, Brugha, TS, Demyttenaere, K, de Girolamo, G, Haro, JM, Jin, R, Karam, E, Kovess-Masfety, V, Levinson, D, Medina Mora, ME, Ono, Y, Ormel, J, Pennell, B-E, Posada-Villa, J, Sampson, NA, Williams, D, Kessler, RC (2009). Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry 66, 785795.Google Scholar
Singh-Manoux, A, Ferrie, JE, Chandola, T, Marmot, M (2004). Socioeconomic trajectories across the life course and health outcomes in midlife: evidence for the accumulation hypothesis? International Journal of Epidemiology 33, 10721079.Google Scholar
Stansfeld, SA, Head, J, Fuhrer, R, Wardle, J, Cattell, V (2003). Social inequalities in depressive symptoms and physical functioning in the Whitehall II study: exploring a common cause explanation. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 57, 361367.Google Scholar
Stansfeld, SA, Head, J, Marmot, MG (1998). Explaining social class differences in depression and well-being. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 33, 19.Google Scholar
Taylor, MG (2008). Timing, accumulation, and the black/white disability gap in later life: a test of weathering. Research on Aging 30, 226250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoits, PA (1999). Sociological approaches to mental illness. In A Handbook for the Study of Mental Health: Social Contexts, Theories, and Systems (ed. Horwitz, A. V. and Scheid, T. L.), pp. 121138. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Tiffin, PA, Pearce, MS, Parker, L (2005). Social mobility over the lifecourse and self reported mental health at age 50: prospective cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59, 870872.Google Scholar
Turner, RJ, Marino, F (1994). Social support and social structure: a descriptive epidemiology. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 35, 193212.Google Scholar
Turner, RJ, Wheaton, B, Lloyd, DA (1995). The epidemiology of social stress. American Sociological Review 60, 104125.Google Scholar
Vink, D, Aartsen, MJ, Schoevers, RA (2008). Risk factors for anxiety and depression in the elderly: a review. Journal of Affective Disorders 106, 2944.Google Scholar
Wiggins, RD, Schofield, P, Sacker, A, Head, J, Bartley, M (2004). Social position and minor psychiatric morbidity over time in the British Household Panel Survey 1991–1998. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 58, 779787.Google Scholar
Wiles, NJ, Peters, TJ, Leon, DA, Lewis, G (2005). Birth weight and psychological distress at age 45-51 years: results from the Aberdeen Children of the 1950s Cohort Study. British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H-U, Beesdo, K, Bittner, A, Goodwin, RD (2003). Depressive episodes: evidence for a causal role of primary anxiety disorders? European Psychiatry 18, 384393.Google Scholar
Zigmond, AS, Snaith, RP (1983). The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 67, 361370.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Distribution of common mental disorders and baseline characteristics across the study waves

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Age trajectories in common mental disorders by baseline social class and adjusted for gender.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Disorder-specific age trajectories by social class and adjusted for gender.

Figure 3

Table 2. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for common mental disorders: sensitivity analyses